Main

We all appreciate the substantial differences among our friends and colleagues in their ability to see, think and act, and such variability introduces a rich diversity of culture and lifestyle into our society. In the neuroscience of human behaviour and cognition, inter-individual differences are often treated as a source of 'noise' and therefore discarded through averaging data from a group of participants. Moreover, university students of industrialized Western countries are typically the participants in many psychology and neuroscience studies1. Despite the narrow selection of human diversity in such experiments, it is widely assumed that the conclusions drawn from a small sample generalize to the entire population. However, inter-individual differences can be exploited to understand the cognitive processes underlying such behaviours2.

Figure 1 illustrates the sort of dataset that is typically used in behavioural experiments, in which responses — such as reaction times, perceptual thresholds or blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signals — show differences between two experimental conditions. Researchers typically focus on the change in the mean response associated with an experimental manipulation or behaviour (Fig. 1a). Such averaging of data across participants is performed to reveal underlying effects despite the presence of measurement noise. However, this averaging ignores a large variation in individual responses (Fig. 1a, right panel). In this example, two participants (pink lines) show an opposite trend from that of the other participants and two participants (green lines) show much larger responses than other participants. These differences are typically viewed as measurement errors or as uninteresting peculiarities of individuals, and are therefore discarded. However, if they are highly consistent across different tests, then they are characteristics of the individuals and may ultimately reflect differences in their brain function. Moreover, as Fig. 1b makes clear, systematic patterns of inter-individual differences (in this case, half the sample showing an opposite response to that of the other half) can be dissociated from differences in mean activity (which are absent in this example).

Figure 1: Examples of typical average and individual responses across two conditions.
figure 1

a | The mean responses for two conditions are illustrated in the left panel. In this example, the response in condition B is significantly larger than in condition A (P < 0.01). Error bars indicate one standard error of the mean (n = 12). Individual data comprising the mean responses are shown by lines in the right panel. Although the overall trend is consistent with the mean values (purple lines), some participants showed opposite trends (pink lines) and others showed much higher responses (green lines). Such inter-individual differences are masked by averaging but could be attributable to variability of brain function. b | The mean responses between conditions A and B do not show a difference. However, the underlying individual data could be divided into two groups of participants showing opposite trends (orange and purple lines). In such cases, the mean results would be uninformative, but specifically studying the cause of the opposite trends between the two groups could reveal relevant brain structures.

In some areas of psychology, such as personality and intelligence research, the main focus has been on inter-individual differences. However, this potentially powerful approach has been almost completely neglected for many years in studies of the neural basis of more basic cognitive functions, such as perception and motor control. These studies have mostly focused on commonalities across individuals and thus often neglect inter-individual differences. This may reflect the fact that, in everyday life, inter-individual differences in basic functions such as vision and action are less noticeable (unless there is a clear pathological deficit) than differences in personality traits or intelligence, and this has lead to an assumption that the neural bases of such basic functions are less variable across individuals.

In this Perspective, we argue that a large amount of phenotypic information about the neural basis of human behaviour and cognition can be obtained by specifically studying inter-individual variability. As visual and motor cortices are arguably the most well-characterized regions in neuroscience, studying the neural basis of inter-individual differences in perception and motor behaviour could be a fruitful approach to understanding how structural differences affect the capacity of the brain regions involved.

Methods that provide measures of neural activity (such as functional MRI (fMRI)3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12, electroencephalography (EEG)13,14,15,16,17,18,19, positron emission tomography20,21,22,23 and magnetic resonance spectroscopy24,25,26) have been used to study the neural bases of individual differences in cognition. However, here we focus on the recently growing body of evidence that inter-individual variability in a wide range of human behaviours can be predicted from the structure of grey matter and white matter tracts of the human brain measured with MRI (Box 1).

Most anatomical MRI studies have focused on differences in brain structure between groups of experts in a particular domain and groups of non-experts. People with specialized skills such as musicians27,28, London taxi drivers29, or Italian bilinguals30 have markedly developed brain structures in specific brain regions associated with their expertise. But experts are by definition extremes in the general population, and there is increasing evidence that anatomical MRI can detect more subtle inter-individual differences even within groups of non-experts. We therefore review studies that specifically examine inter-individual differences within a population of healthy adults, rather than the extensive literature comparing neuroanatomical differences between particular groups (for example, those with neurological and psychiatric diseases) and healthy volunteers.

Motor behaviour and decision making

Perhaps the simplest action that can be taken in response to a visual stimulus is to press a button. Reaction times vary according to whether the responding hand is contralateral or ipsilateral to the stimulated visual field, which has been attributed to timing delays associated with the need for inter-hemispheric neural signalling when the responding hand is contralateral to the stimulated visual field. Inter-individual differences in this interhemispheric transmission time are reflected in the microstructural integrity of the corpus callosum, assessed by diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), in healthy volunteers31 and individuals with schizophrenia32.

Other simple motor paradigms also show inter-individual variability associated with differences in brain structure. For example, variability in choice reaction time (that is, the time taken to indicate a choice, usually by pressing a button) across individuals correlates with fractional anisotropy of the optic radiation33 (Fig. 2a) as measured by DTI (Box 1). With regard to more complex motor coordination, inter-individual differences in the skill of a bimanual coordination task are reflected in the differences in the integrity of the part of the corpus callosum that links supplementary motor areas34. Moreover, inter-individual variability in the ability to select the correct response in the presence of response conflict correlates with the grey matter density of the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA)35 (Fig. 2b). Thus, it seems that inter-individual variation in both the initiation and the cognitive control of simple and complex motor tasks is reflected in the structural anatomy of the brain.

Figure 2: Structural bases of inter-individual differences in action and decision making.
figure 2

a | The speed of reaction time in making a visual choice correlates with the fractional anisotropy (a measure of white matter integrity) of the right optic radiation (indicated by the white box). b | Grey matter density of the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA) correlates with the degree of the response conflict effect. The scatter plot shows the correlation in the condition in which conflicting response tendencies were elicited consciously (because the conflicting stimuli were only weakly masked). c | Connection strength between the pre-SMA (upper green area in the left panel) and striatum (lower green area in the left panel) correlates with individuals' ability to adjust the speed–accuracy trade-off. Part a is modified, with permission, from Ref. 33 © 2005 National Academy of Sciences. Part b is modified, with permission, from Ref. 35 © 2011 MIT Press. Part c is modified, with permission, from Ref. 42 © 2010 National Academy of Sciences.

In decision making, fast decisions often come at the cost of reduced accuracy36,37. This phenomenon is termed the speed–accuracy trade-off and has been observed in many decision-making tasks38,39,40,41. Thus, when faster responses are required, one needs to flexibly adjust the decision criterion. There are considerable inter-individual differences in the ability to flexibly adjust the speed–accuracy trade-off, and this variability correlates with connection strengths between the pre-SMA and striatum, as measured using DTI42 (Fig. 2c). This finding is compatible with earlier functional MRI studies showing, first, that the areas connected by this pathway are activated by cues that indicate higher demands for response speed; and second, that activation in these areas correlates with the ability to switch between cautious and risky behaviours43. These studies of inter-individual differences also suggest that the cortico–basal ganglia circuitry regulates the speed–accuracy trade-off.

As these examples illustrate, studies of inter-individual differences are not limited to explaining the neural basis of performance differences between individual participants, but can be exploited to reveal the circuitry associated with a particular cognitive function.

Perception

The existence of inter-individual variability in psychophysical thresholds for sensory discrimination has been known for many years, as has that of inter-individual variability in the size of components of early sensory processing pathways, such as the lateral geniculate nucleus and primary visual cortex in the visual pathway44,45. The size of the surface area of the primary visual cortex that is devoted to processing visual signals from a particular part of the visual field varies as a function of eccentricity from fixation, and this relationship can be characterized by the cortical magnification factor46. In humans, inter-individual variability in the visual acuity threshold correlates with this cortical magnification factor, establishing a link between variability in perception and brain structure47. However, such a relationship was established in studies in which both perception and physical stimulation varied at the same time. More recent work has sought to dissociate perception and physical stimulation, establishing a link between sensory awareness and brain structure, as discussed below.

Perceptual rivalry. When visual input has conflicting interpretations (for example, the Necker cube), conscious perception can alternate spontaneously between the competing interpretations. Such spontaneous fluctuations of conscious perception can be used to delineate the neural basis of conscious perception48, because the subjective perception fluctuates while the physical stimulation is constant. Surprisingly, there is great inter-individual variability in the rate at which these spontaneous alternations occur49,50. A recent study tested whether an individual's 'perceptual switch rate' was reflected in their cortical thickness, local grey matter volume and/or white matter integrity. All of these measures of brain structure converged to show that the structure of the bilateral superior parietal lobes (SPLs) can account for inter-individual variability in perceptual switch rate. Specifically, individuals with a fast switch rate have thicker and larger volume SPLs than individuals with a slow switch rate, and the white matter underlying the SPLs has a higher integrity. This correlation of SPL structure with individuals' switch rates suggests that these bilateral regions are involved in triggering spontaneous perceptual switches51. Furthermore, the anterior part of the right intraparietal sulcus (IPS) shows an opposite correlation — that is, the larger the grey matter volume of this area, the slower the switch rate (Fig. 3a). The opposing influences of the SPLs and the anterior IPS suggest that these areas might have complementary roles, with the SPLs detecting possible alternative perceptual interpretations, and the anterior IPS sustaining the current percept52.

Figure 3: Structural bases of inter-individual differences in conscious perception.
figure 3

a | Structural correlates of inter-individual differences in the duration of one percept in a perceptual rivalry task (in which a single visual input can have conflicting interpretations) (left panel). A larger posterior superior parietal lobe (pSPL) was associated with a slower rate of switching between competing interpretations of a visual input, whereas a larger anterior superior parietal lobe (aSPL) was associated with a faster switch rate. Data in the middle and right panels are from Ref. 51 and Ref. 52, respectively. b | The surface areas of visual cortical areas V1, V2 and V3 from two example participants (left panels). A larger V1 was associated with weaker Ebbinghaus and Ponzo illusions (right panels). c | A structural correlate of metacognitive ability (left panel). Statistical T-maps for positive ('hot' colour map: red, orange and yellow) correlations and negative ('cool' colour map: blue) correlations between grey matter volume and metacognitive ability are projected onto an inflated cortical surface. Better metacognitive abilities were associated with a larger Brodmann area 10 (BA10), an area in the rostral prefrontal cortex (right panel). The left panel of part a is reproduced, with permission, from Ref. 52 © 2011 Cell Press. Part b is reproduced, with permission, from Ref. 60 © 2011 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. All rights reserved. Part c is modified, with permission, from Ref. 80 © 2010 American Association for the Advancement of Science.

The parietal cortex areas in which structural variation is associated with perceptual switch rate are similar to the regions that are activated when perceptual switches occur53,54,55,56. Previously, it was unclear whether the functional role of these parietal structures was related to the active triggering of perceptual switches53,57 or instead to sustaining the current percept58,59. The anatomical studies described above, corroborated by complementary transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) experiments51,52, suggest that the parietal cortex contains different subregions associated with both proposed functions.

Despite earlier findings53,54,55,56 that prefrontal cortical regions are also activated during perceptual switches, anatomical studies51,52 have not found a neural correlate of perceptual switch rate in prefrontal regions. Whether this dissociation indicates that inter-individual differences in switch rate are attributed only to the parietal, but not to the frontal, cortex remains to be determined, as the inability to identify structural correlates in the prefrontal cortex could simply be a matter of statistical power.

Sensory awareness. Subjective awareness of physically identical visual stimuli can also vary across different individuals. For example, individuals with colour blindness perceive colours differently to those with normal vision. This raises the possibility that even individuals with normal vision may show variability in how they perceive the world. Although it is difficult to directly compare the subjective experiences of different people, inter-individual differences in the perceived strength of a perceptual illusion — whereby physically identical stimuli produce perceptually different appearances depending on their local context — can be quantitatively compared. In a study that compared individuals' susceptibility to geometrical visual illusions (the Ponzo and Ebbinghaus illusions), just such variability in illusion strength was found60. Moreover, the strength of the illusion correlated negatively with the size of early retinotopic visual area V1, but not visual area V2 and visual area V3 (Ref. 60) (Fig. 3b; see Box 2 for possible mechanisms mediating such correlation).

Retinotopic mapping techniques using fMRI allow delineation of borders between early visual areas, and close relationships between anatomical folding patterns and retinotopic representations of early visual areas have been reported61. By contrast, a purely anatomical measure of the surface area of the peri-calcarine cortex (where V1 is located) does not correlate with the illusion strengths60. This suggests that the size of the surface areas of visual regions (as determined by fMRI62) reflect an individual's visual experience much more sensitively than crude gyral and sulcal anatomy alone. In non-visual cortical regions, it is generally difficult to estimate the size of the surface areas of functionally segregated areas because it is difficult to unambiguously delineate the borders between them. For subcortical regions, however, the size can be unambiguously estimated on the basis of structural MRI measurements (for example, the size of the amygdala or hippocampus63,64,65). It is thus possible to establish the relationship between the size of such regions and inter-individual variability in cognition and behaviour. For example, the size of the amygdala correlates with inter-individual differences in memory66, social phobia67 and social network size68.

The grey matter density in the calcarine sulcus (plus the auditory cortex) is also associated with inter-individual differences in synaesthetic experiences, in which a particular stimulus evokes a sensory experience in addition to the modality-typical sensory experience69. Grapheme–colour synaesthesia — in which letters and numbers are associated with certain colours — is one of the most common types of synaesthesia. There are two types of grapheme–colour synaesthetes: 'projectors' see the associated colour in the external world, whereas 'associators' experience the colours in their mind. Projectors have more grey matter in the calcarine sulcus and the prefrontal cortex than associators, and associators have more grey matter in the hippocampus and angular gyrus than projectors. These findings suggest that the experiences of associated colours in the external world in projector synaesthetes may be mediated by the primary sensory cortex.

Metacognition. Metacognition, or 'cognition about cognition', refers to the ability to comment or report on one's own mental state70 and is often considered the touchstone of the presence of consciousness in humans71,72,73,74 and animals75,76. In the context of sensory processing, we can be correct or incorrect in our perceptual judgments, but we can also provide a metacognitive estimate of our confidence each time we make such a judgment. The ability of different individuals to accurately link confidence and performance — that is, their metacognitive ability — can be formulated and quantified using the so-called type 2 performance in signal detection theory77,78.

Substantial inter-individual differences exist in metacognitive ability79. A recent voxel-based morphometry (VBM) study revealed that metacognitive ability — defined operationally as the ability to appropriately link insight (confidence) to objective performance in a perceptual decision-making task — is reflected in the grey matter volume of the rostral prefrontal cortex and precuneus80 (Fig. 3c). Fractional anisotropy in the genu of the corpus callosum also correlates with metacognitive ability, suggesting the importance of white matter connections linking the rostral prefrontal cortex to other cortical regions80.

These three studies of inter-individual differences in visual awareness suggest that, although the architecture of the basic visual pathways must be similar among healthy individuals, differences in their subjective experiences can be attributed to regionally specific morphological differences in the brain.

Attention. The ability to control attention to relevant tasks varies considerably across individuals. The attention network test (ANT) is widely used to assess three dissociable aspects of attention: executive control, orienting and alerting81,82. Cortical thickness in several brain regions is positively correlated with the executive control and alerting (but not the orienting) components of attention83. Specifically, the executive control component of attention is reflected in the thickness of the anterior cingulate cortex, the right inferior frontal gyrus and left medial frontal areas extending to the frontal pole and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. By contrast, the alerting component of attention is negatively correlated with the thickness of the left superior parietal lobe extending to the precuneus. These findings of structure–cognition relationships illustrate that individual differences in attentional networks can be mapped onto differences in grey matter brain structure.

Functional neuroimaging studies have implicated areas in attentional control that are broadly similar to those that show inter-individual variability in structure. For example, the anterior cingulate cortex and the right inferior frontal gyrus are activated during the ANT84 and in tasks that involve conflict monitoring and response inhibition (types of executive control)85,86, and the alerting component of the ANT is associated with activation of the left superior parietal cortex. However, dissociations between structural and functional findings are also observed. For example, the alerting component of the ANT induced stronger activation in brain regions the thickness of which does not correlate with attentional performance, such as the right superior temporal gyrus84. Such differences may arise because the structural MRI analyses reviewed here focus on the variability that gives rise to inter-individual differences in behaviour, whereas fMRI studies typically reveal the most consistent activation of brain regions across individuals. A direct comparison of inter-individual differences in fMRI signals with VBM within the same participants will be important for understanding the relationship between functional and structural MRI results87,88,89,90.

Intelligence and personality

Economic motivation and decision making. In addition to simple decision-making behaviour (discussed above), more complex motivation and decision-making processes show correlations with brain structure. For example, inter-individual differences in delay-discounting behaviour (the tendency to prefer receiving small, immediate rewards over large, delayed rewards) are correlated with white matter integrity in frontal and temporal lobe white matter tracts in 9–23-year-olds91.

The idea that delay-discounting behaviour in children is reflected in the development of connections within the prefrontal cortex91 is consistent with the involvement of the prefrontal cortex in this type of behaviour. For example, higher BOLD responses are observed in the prefrontal cortex when adult participants choose a delayed rather than an immediate reward92. Similarly, the association of the integrity of white matter tracts in the temporal lobe with delay-discounting behaviour91 is consistent with studies showing that rats with hippocampal damage choose immediate rewards in delay-discounting tasks93.

Intelligence and information processing speed. In contrast to the study of motor behaviour or visual perception, the psychology of human intelligence has a long tradition of specifically investigating individual differences in humans, and there is a rich literature on this topic. The cognitive neuroscience of intelligence is beyond the scope of this Perspective and has been reviewed elsewhere94.

Some examples of the correlation of brain structure with measures of intelligence include structural MRI studies that show that inter-individual variability in intelligence correlates with cortical thickness95,96,97,98 and white matter integrity, as assessed with DTI99,100,101,102. Moreover, a global network parameter that is derived from white matter tractography and reflects network efficiency is correlated with intelligence103, suggesting that the degree to which white matter connectivity can support efficient information processing may be important for intelligence. Information processing speed, assessed by simple reaction tasks, is associated with intelligence104,105. In healthy older people, a general factor associated with white matter integrity (across eight major white matter tracts quantified using probabilistic tractography techniques) predicts information processing speed106.

Personality. In psychology, many questionnaires have been devised to measure personality traits. Personality psychologists often use a model called the 'Big Five'107 to describe the fundamental dimensions of personality traits; these comprise neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness and conscientiousness. Differences in these broad factors and their narrower facets across individuals have consequences on everyday behaviour108 and in cognitive tasks109,110,111. Moreover, up to half of the variability in these five personality traits is heritable112,113, suggesting that inter-individual differences in these traits have biological bases in the brain114. In addition, the high degree of heritability in such psychological constructs indicates the biological relevance and validity of psychometrics, and thus motivates further investigation of their relationship to the brain.

As with research on intelligence, personality psychology has a long tradition of studying inter-individual differences. However, investigations into the structural bases of the Big Five personality traits have started only recently (Fig. 4). One study found that anatomical variability in specific brain regions predicts inter-individual differences in personality traits115. Specifically, extraversion correlates positively with grey matter volume in the medial orbitofrontal cortex. By contrast, neuroticism correlates negatively with grey matter volume in the right dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and left medial temporal lobe, and correlates positively with grey matter volume in the mid cingulate cortex. Agreeableness correlates positively with grey matter volume in the posterior cingulate cortex and correlates negatively with that in the superior temporal sulcus and fusiform gyrus. Conscientiousness correlates positively with grey matter volume in the lateral prefrontal cortex. However, openness does not show statistically significant correlations with grey matter volume in any given region. Similarly, brain structure correlates of more specific traits such as impulsivity (as measured by the Barret Impulsivity Scale) have been found in the orbitofrontal cortex116. Variation in the volume of the orbitofrontal cortex is also related to variability in emotion regulation and affect117.

Figure 4: Brain structure correlates of higher cognitive functions.
figure 4

a | Grey matter (GM; left panel) and white matter (WM; right panel) correlates of general intelligence. Greater grey matter and white matter volumes in specific brain areas are associated with higher intelligence. b | Grey matter correlates of the Big Five traits. Grey matter volume in specific cortical areas correlates with scores on a specific trait. PFC, prefrontal cortex. Part a is reproduced, with permission, from Ref. 125 © 2004 Elsevier. Part b is modified, with permission, from Ref. 115 © 2010 Sage Publications.

Neuroanatomical investigations into the biological basis of personality traits measured by self-report questionnaires may in the future provide a solid ground for traditional personality psychology, which currently relies heavily on semantic description and the participants' ability to estimate their own personality (that is, metacognition). Furthermore, questionnaire-based approaches to probing brain structure may allow a set of questions to be devised the answers to which predict the size of brain structures of interest. For example, it is possible that an individual's answer to the question “Are you scared of snakes?” may predict the size of the amygdala — a key region for processing fear memories — of that individual. Such approaches may reveal links between a brain region and its function in everyday life. A crucial challenge for brain structure analyses will be the ability to predict an individual's ability or personality better than self-report questionnaires. Self reports reflect a range of cognitive biases (such as overestimation known as the Kruger-Dunning effect118), and structural MRI data could potentially offer less biased information about an individual's personality trait. At present, no direct comparison between a questionnaire and brain structure measurements has been reported. However, such comparisons will be crucial to test brain-based descriptions of personality traits in future.

Social cognition. Humans are social animals, but they show variability in the degree to which they engage in social activity. The volume of the amygdala correlates with the size and complexity of social networks in adult humans68, and there are relationships — albeit weaker — between variables reflecting the structure of an individual's social network and cortical thickness in three cortical areas connected with the amygdala68. Thus, even complex psychological concepts such as the construction and maintenance of social networks have a correlate in brain structure.

Genetic and plasticity effects on anatomy

Studies in monozygotic and dizygotic twins have shown that grey matter volumes of the prefrontal and temporal cortex are strongly influenced by genetic factors119, whereas other areas are less strongly affected. This suggests that the heritability of a particular cognitive function depends on the extent to which the relevant cortical regions are influenced by genetic factors120,121,122. Genetic factors contribute to cortical thickness and surface area size independently123. Such genetic contributions to variations in brain structure may underlie heritability of cognitive abilities such as the intelligence quotient (IQ)97,124,125,126,127,128.

Although early stages of brain development are mediated by genetic programmes129,130, later stages of development, as well as brain organization and brain maturation, result from interactions between genetic and environmental factors131,132. Indeed, recent VBM findings indicate that brain structure is not determined solely by genetic factors but is extensively modulated by experiences such as prolonged training133,134,135,136,137,138. For example, training on a visual motor coordination task, such as learning to juggle, has a measurable effect on grey matter volume of visual motion processing area V5 and the posterior parietal cortex133,135 as well as white matter integrity of neighbouring fibres that presumably mediate visuo-spatial transformation138. Moreover, such structural plasticity can be demonstrated in the adult brain across training periods as short as 90 minutes over 2 weeks139.

Moreover, compared with carefully matched, illiterate controls, individuals who have learned to read for the first time as adults have greater grey matter volume in the bilateral angular, dorsal occipital, middle temporal, left supramarginal and superior temporal gyri140 (see also Ref. 141). These areas are associated with crucial functions for literacy such as semantic, phonological and high-level visual processing142,143 and highlight the possibility that plastic structural changes in the adult human brain are associated with training.

Correlations in performance across tasks

In structural MRI studies, such as those discussed above, measurements of brain structure can be performed separately from measurements of behavioural performance, which can occur outside the MRI scanner using conventional behavioural or psychological tasks. This separation offers the opportunity to study the relationship between brain regions and relatively static properties of an individual's characteristics, such as personality traits. By contrast, functional neuroimaging studies using fMRI and EEG require task designs that evoke activation in the brain in a manner that is relevant to the trait of interest (for example, Refs 4, 10), which can be difficult. Thus, an important advantage of neuroanatomical studies of inter-individual differences (over studies using fMRI or EEG) is that they allow one to link an individual's performance or trait as measured in an ecologically valid environment (that is, outside an MRI scanner) to brain structure measurements obtained in an MRI scanner.

It is conceivable that inter-individual differences in performance on a single task can be mapped onto the structure of a single brain region, but such a simplistic notion of a one-to-one mapping between a cognitive function and the structure of a brain region needs to be examined carefully. As has been shown in many functional neuroimaging studies, a single region can be involved in a broad range of tasks. Thus, it is unlikely that there is always one core region that is crucial for a particular cognitive function. Instead, a region with a structure that correlates with a behavioural measure needs to be interpreted in the context of the known functions of the region and its role in other, related behavioural tasks.

The separation of behavioural measurements from structural measurements of brain anatomy also permits multiple tasks to be administered to the same set of participants. One successful approach to analysing the correlations between related cognitive tasks uses principal component analysis or factor analysis. For example, research into the principal factors in working memory and attentional tasks has characterized how different aspects of working memory and attention correlate with each other144. Moreover, the components derived from such an analysis can subsequently be used to determine whether the structure of particular brain areas is associated with each component in a VBM analysis. Such a combined approach of factor analysis and VBM has been successfully applied to face perception in individuals with developmental prosopagnosia145. This multivariate approach to analysing behavioural data is a promising way to address correlations between different tasks, as it can potentially reveal common underlying components and any neural substrates. Such an approach may provide new insights into how different cognitive functions are related to each other and which regions underlie those functions.

A disadvantage of studies that relate brain structure to behaviour is that the data are temporally unchanging, except when specifically studied through characterization of anatomical changes associated with learning or development. This contrasts with the temporally dynamic nature of functional neuroimaging data, which allows researchers to investigate how activation patterns change over time across the brain and to characterize functional interactions between distant brain regions. However, there have been attempts to reveal network structures in the brain by examining the structural covariation of brain regions146,147,148. Such analyses of structural covariance across the brain may offer the possibility of characterizing individuals in terms of network strengths instead of voxel-based local volumes.

Limitations of current research

Microstructural basis of MRI. Understanding the cellular basis of local changes in grey matter volume is necessary for a better interpretation of VBM studies. However, the microstructure and cellular events that give rise to a global quantity that can be measured by structural MRI remain poorly understood. One possibility is that differences in grey matter volume reflect underlying synaptogenesis and dendritic arborization, which in rats are known to vary markedly between animals reared in environments of different levels of complexity149,150. Such experience-dependent formation and elimination of synapses continues into adulthood in rodents151.

The radial unit hypothesis put forward by Rakic suggests that neurons within the same cortical column have a common developmental origin and migrate along the same pathway from the ventricular zone131,152,153. Before migration starts, progenitor cells undergo symmetrical cell division and increase the number of the radial columns in the ventricular zone. This process has consequences for the allocation of cortical columns and regional surface areas in the mature cortex. By contrast, the thickness of the cortex is determined by the number of cells produced by asymmetrical cell divisions within the ontogenic columns152,153. These two stages of cellular events (that is, symmetrical and asymmetrical cell divisions) are likely to be controlled by different sets of genes. Consistent with this model, cortical thickness and surface area size are independently heritable in humans123. The radial unit hypothesis provides an important framework for understanding how individual differences in cortical thickness and surface area may be determined by genetic and developmental processes.

However, a direct link between microstructures and macrostructures has not been established in the human brain. A histological study directly compared whether histopathological measurements of resected temporal lobe tissue correlated with grey matter density as used in typical VBM studies154. However, none of the histological measures — including neuronal density — showed a clear relationship with the grey matter volume154. We suggest that determining the microscopic neuronal structures that give rise to macroscopic structural differences will be an important step towards understanding how volumetric measures of the brain structure translate into differences in computational capacity. However, different populations should be compared with caution, as the microstructural events that correlate with a cognitive function may differ between, for example, healthy and diseased or young and old individuals. Some VBM studies show a negative correlation between grey matter volume and cognitive performance (Box 3). To interpret such seemingly paradoxical results, it will be important to investigate how macroscopic volumetric measures are reflected in microstructures at the cellular level.

From correlation to causation. In general, when we try to relate inter-individual differences to brain structure using VBM, we face a massive multiple-comparison problem. This requires both a large sample of participants (typically tens to hundreds of participants) and fairly high correlations between brain structure and behavioural data to achieve conventional levels of statistical significance with appropriate correction for multiple comparisons. The upper limit of any correlation is constrained by the reliability of both behavioural and MRI measurements, and this reliability can be assessed by test–retest consistency in the same participants. Thus, improving the behavioural measurements could improve the sensitivity of VBM analysis.

As with studies investigating brain activation using fMRI, VBM studies of brain anatomy are intrinsically correlational. They can only show an association between the structure of a particular brain region and some behavioural performance. Such correlational associations do not necessarily imply causal relationships, and there are also chances of false discoveries due to the highly multidimensional nature of such correlational analyses.

To complement such correlational analyses, we suggest that intervention studies using brain stimulation techniques such as TMS and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) can provide independent support for a causal link between structure and function. For example, in the VBM studies of perceptual rivalry described above51,52 (Fig. 3a), we first identified cortical regions the structure (that is, cortical thickness) of which correlated with individuals' switch rates between competing perceptions, and then used TMS to confirm that those regions have a causal role in generating switches. Correlations between the thickness or size of a brain structure and performance on a task of interest can be used to generate new hypotheses as to which brain areas might be crucial for the performance of the task. The functional involvement of those areas in the task can subsequently be confirmed (or disproved) by disrupting the function of those regions with intervention methods such as TMS and tDCS.

It is often assumed that there is a close relationship between changes in brain structure measured using structural MRI, and changes in brain activity measured using fMRI. However, such a close relationship remains to be demonstrated. The cellular basis of the changes underlying VBM findings associated with structural MRI is uncertain, and BOLD fMRI signals can in certain circumstances be dissociated from patterns of neuronal spiking155,156,157,158,159. It is thus conceivable that individual differences in anatomical structure and BOLD activity may be dissociated for components of brain networks associated with certain behaviours. We suggest that understanding the relationship between inter-individual differences in brain structure and brain function may be a rich area for future research.

Conclusions and future directions

Investigations of inter-individual differences in human behaviour show that surprisingly rich information about individuals is encoded in their brain anatomy and can be measured using non-invasive structural MRI. There has been a rapid growth in the number of studies examining inter-individual differences in human behaviour and its association with structural features of the brain. Associations between white and grey matter anatomy and behaviour are not confined to motor behaviour and motor learning, but extend into domains of sensory perception and many areas of higher-order cognition. This has identified a number of crucial issues for future work to address: first, cross-sectional studies do not distinguish between the possibilities that brain structure varies in response to behavioural variability or vice versa. Longitudinal or interventional studies are required to help parse causality between brain structure and behaviour. Second, the time course of structural plasticity needs to be addressed. In the motor domain, a surprising amount of short-term plasticity (over a few weeks) in brain structure is apparent. The degree to which other aspects of inter-individual variability in perception or higher cognitive processes are susceptible to similar plasticity is an intriguing topic for future development. Third, the predictive power of brain anatomy for evaluating brain structural correlates of individual differences needs to be established more fully. Recent efforts to predict clinical phenotype from brain anatomy in autism spectrum disorders160 and Alzheimer's disease161 highlight the potential for using brain anatomy as an aid to clinical diagnosis. This raises the question of how much behavioural variability in healthy humans can be predicted on an individual (or group) basis from brain anatomy; and whether such variability can (or should) have an application. Fourth, the microstructural basis of MRI structural differences needs to be determined. The relatively poor understanding of the structural basis underlying differences detected by VBM and other MRI-based techniques will remain a limitation until its cellular basis is better characterized. The lack of inter-individual variability in animal models may present a difficulty in addressing this question.

In this Perspective, we have argued that inter-individual differences in behaviour, often discarded by averaging data across participants, can be a rich and important source of information and can be exploited to reveal the neural basis of human cognition and behaviour in general. We suggest that the future directions outlined above will be important areas of research to build the foundations of this emerging area of research.