Main

Pain is a complex sensory and emotional experience that can vary widely between people and even within an individual depending on the context and meaning of the pain and the psychological state of the person. Cognitive and emotional factors have a surprisingly important influence on pain perception. A negative expectation can completely reverse the analgesic effect of a clinical dose of the opioid agonist remifentanil1, whereas the expectation of pain relief is an important component of placebo analgesia2. Clinical and experimental studies show that even a simple psychological manipulation, such as distraction, can have a powerful effect on our perception of pain3. Our emotional state also has an enormous influence on pain; a negative emotional state increases pain, whereas a positive state lowers pain4. Not surprisingly, complex emotional states such as empathy, which incorporate emotional and cognitive factors, alter the way an individual feels pain5. Brain imaging studies have allowed us to examine the neural basis of psychological modulation of pain (Box 1). These studies reveal that activity in afferent pain pathways is altered by the attentional state, positive and negative emotions, empathy and the administration of a placebo6. Additionally, imaging studies show that psychological factors activate intrinsic modulatory systems in the brain, including those involved in opioid-related pain relief. Furthermore, multiple descending pain modulatory systems are implicated in pain relief, with attentional states and emotions activating different systems in the brain.

In parallel with our increased understanding of the circuitry underlying the psychological modulation of pain, recent evidence shows that chronic pain can lead to anatomical and functional alterations in this same circuitry, resulting not only in pain but also in altered cognition and affect. In Eastern cultures, the power of the mind to control one's physical state with the use of practices such as meditation and yoga has been recognized for centuries. Today, people in the Western world have begun to adopt these techniques to reduce stress and control pain, and many pain sufferers now recognize the potential therapeutic effects of psychological procedures. In line with patient reports, new experimental studies show that interventions such as meditation not only decrease pain but also have powerful protective effects on brain grey matter and connectivity within pain modulatory circuits7,8,9,10,11,12. The wealth of new studies showing alterations in the brains of patients with chronic pain can now be integrated with our increased understanding of the brain circuitry involved in the psychological modulation of pain, allowing us to hypothesize a negative-feedback loop between impaired pain modulatory circuitry and pain processing, leading not only to increased chronic pain but also to cognitive and emotional deficits that are comorbid with the pain (Fig. 1).

Figure 1: Feedback loops between pain, emotions and cognition.
figure 1

Pain can have a negative effect on emotions and on cognitive function. Conversely, a negative emotional state can lead to increased pain, whereas a positive state can reduce pain. Similarly, cognitive states such as attention and memory can either increase or decrease pain. Of course, emotions and cognition can also reciprocally interact. The minus sign refers to a negative effect and the plus sign refers to a positive effect.

PowerPoint slide

Brain regions involved in the experience of pain

Multiple pathways in the CNS are involved in pain processing. Human brain imaging studies have revealed consistent cortical and subcortical networks that are activated by pain, including sensory, limbic and associative regions. The brain areas most commonly activated by noxious stimuli in human brain imaging studies are the primary somatosensory cortex (S1), secondary somatosensory cortex (S2), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), insula, prefrontal cortex (PFC), thalamus and cerebellum13 (Fig. 2). Neural activation in these areas is consistent with anatomical and electrophysiological studies that show possible afferent nociceptive connectivity to these regions13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20. Other regions have also been shown to have nociceptive input through anatomical and electrophysiological studies, and these findings have been confirmed by brain imaging studies. These regions, which include the nucleus accumbens and amygdala21,22, probably receive nociceptive input through spinoparabrachial–amygdala projections23, as well as the periaqueductal grey (PAG)24, which receives nociceptive input through spinoreticular pathways25.

Figure 2: Afferent pain pathways include multiple brain regions.
figure 2

Afferent nociceptive information enters the brain from the spinal cord. Afferent spinal pathways include the spinothalamic, spinoparabrachio–amygdaloid and spinoreticulo–thalamic pathways. Nociceptive information from the thalamus is projected to the insula, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), primary somatosensory cortex (S1) and secondary somatosensory cortex (S2), whereas information from the amygdala (AMY) is projected to the basal ganglia (BG). See the main text for references. PAG, periaqueductal grey; PB, parabrachial nucleus; PFC, prefrontal cortex.

PowerPoint slide

As would be expected, the multiple pain-related brain pathways are important for different aspects of the pain experience. Pain involves sensations with qualities such as stinging, burning or aching and has identifiable locations and durations. However, what makes pain 'pain' is usually the affective component of the experience — that is, how unpleasant it is. It is the unpleasantness that motivates the individual to engage in a behaviour, whether it is to flee, fight or freeze. In some cases, a painful sensation can be experienced as pleasurable, such as a deep massage, and in that case it creates a positive motivational state. Brain imaging and lesion studies implicate different brain regions in these different aspects of the pain experience. The somatosensory cortices (S1 and S2) encode information about sensory features, such as the location and duration of pain26,27,28,29,30. Alternatively, the ACC and insula, which have long been considered components of the limbic (emotional) part of the brain31,32, are more important for encoding the emotional and motivational aspects of pain. Patients with lesions of these regions show altered emotional responses to pain33,34,35,36, and imaging and stimulation studies show a relationship between emotional and motivational aspects of pain perception and neural activation in the ACC and insula37,38,39,40. Nevertheless, accumulating evidence implicates the insula in both pain sensation and pain affect, with the anterior portion being particularly important for the subjective experience of pain41,42.

Some studies show that a noxious insult to the body is not necessary for the experience of pain or for the activation of pain pathways. A number of studies have found that simply observing another individual in pain activates some pain-related brain regions (see Ref. 43 for a meta-analysis), with such activation being stronger when one observes a loved one in pain rather than a stranger44. The activation of these regions in the absence of a pain stimulus could induce a state of 'priming' in the brain, in which a noxious stimulus given after emotionally priming a subject elicits an enhanced pain experience. This is exactly what was found in a study in which the authors created a state of positive or negative empathy towards someone experiencing pain5. When subjects were positively empathizing with the individual in pain, their own pain experience was enhanced. Similar empathetically enhanced pain responses have been reported in rodents. In another study, the authors produced intriguing evidence that mice are influenced by the pain state of a cage-mate45. When mice see cage-mates in pain, they show increased pain behaviours themselves — a phenomenon the authors call 'emotional contagion'45.

Empathy is a complex cognitive and emotional state that could involve alterations in attention, emotions and expectations. Nevertheless, simpler cognitive manipulations, such as imagining or anticipating a painful experience, can also lead to activation of pain pathways. One study found that imagining a painful hand movement in an amputated arm activated pain pathways, involving the S1, insula and thalamus46. Similarly, a number of studies have shown that when an individual is anticipating the onset of pain, pain-related brain regions that are important for pain sensation, pain affect and pain modulation become activated, including the somatosensory cortices, ACC, insula, PFC and PAG47,48,49,50,51,52,53.

Together, these findings indicate that brain regions receive afferent pain input via multiple routes and that, at least in higher brain regions, there is no 'labelled line' between the peripheral nociceptive input and the final cortical representation of that input. This idea is consistent with evidence of multiple descending modulatory circuits, which either enhance or reduce the afferent signal at multiple regions throughout the neuraxis, from the cortex down to the spinal cord.

Attention and emotion influence pain differently

For centuries there have been anecdotal accounts of individuals apparently experiencing little or no pain in situations that by all accounts should be excruciating. Beecher54 described wounded soldiers who felt little pain from grievous injuries in the battlefield but reacted strongly to minor painful procedures when they returned home. However, Western medicine has traditionally ignored the role of the mind in pain control and has focused on pharmacological treatments of pain. Nevertheless, there is now a growing emphasis on mind–body techniques for controlling pain, with many patients with chronic pain turning to cognitive behavioural therapy, yoga, meditation, hypnosis and relaxation procedures to reduce their pain. These techniques are complex but most have both a cognitive component, such as attentional focus, and an emotional component. Growing evidence shows that these therapies can reduce both chronic and acute pain (for reviews, see Refs 55,56,57).

Both attentional and emotional factors are known to modulate pain perception in the clinic and in the laboratory3,58,59 (Box 2). However, the nature and mechanisms of this modulation differ60,61,62. Focusing on pain increases the perceived intensity of the sensation, whereas a negative emotional state increases the perceived unpleasantness of the pain without altering the intensity4,59,60,61 (Fig. 3). One study found that emotional valence influenced pain ratings and a spinal nociceptive reflex in the same direction but distraction reduced pain while increasing the reflex62. Thus, it appears that different systems may be involved in the modulation of pain by attention and emotions.

Figure 3: Attentional and emotional factors modulate pain perception via different pathways.
figure 3

a | Manipulating the attentional state primarily alters the perceived intensity of the pain sensation without significantly altering the perceived unpleasantness of the pain (top graph). By contrast, altering the mood state alters the perceived unpleasantness of the pain without altering the intensity of the sensation (bottom graph). b | Attention and emotion alter pain via different descending modulatory systems. Emotions (and placebo analgesia) activate circuitry involving the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), prefrontal cortex (PFC) and periaqueductal grey (PAG) (shown in green), whereas attention activates circuitry involving projections from the superior parietal lobe (SPL) to the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) and insula (shown in blue). Grey regions show parts of the ascending pain pathways depicted in Fig. 2. AMY, amygdala; BG, basal ganglia; PB, parabrachial nucleus; RVM, rostroventral medulla; S2, secondary somatosensory cortex. * indicates p < 0.05, and ** indicates p < 0.01. Data used in part a are from Ref. 4.

PowerPoint slide

Human brain imaging studies examining the effects of distraction on pain processing find that when a person focuses on pain, the pain-evoked activity in several cortical areas, including the S1, insula and ACC, is stronger than when a person is distracted from the pain4,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70. However, some of these studies used distracting stimuli that also alter arousal or emotional state64,65,66,67,68, so that the modulatory effects could be due to either attentional or emotional factors. The studies that varied the direction of attention while controlling for emotional state found that pain-evoked activity was modulated by attentional direction only in the insula and S1 (Refs 4,63,69,70), which is consistent with the role of these regions in pain sensation.

Neuroimaging studies evaluating the effects of emotional states on pain processing also find that negative emotional states produced by looking at emotional faces, listening to unpleasant music or smelling unpleasant odours alter pain-evoked cortical activation in a number of brain regions but most consistently in the ACC4,69,71,72,73. The modulation of activity in the ACC is in agreement with evidence that this region is particularly important for pain unpleasantness.

Different modulatory systems are involved in the attentional and emotional control of pain. The finding that attention and emotion have dissimilar effects on pain perception and alter pain-evoked activation of different cortical regions suggests that separate modulatory systems may underlie attentional and emotional influences on pain. The most commonly studied pain modulatory pathways involve projections from the midbrain PAG to brainstem nuclei, including the rostroventral medulla (RVM) and the locus coeruleus, to the dorsal horn of the spinal cord (for reviews, see Refs 74,75). These pathways involve endogenous opioids, noradrenaline and serotonin, and have both inhibitory and excitatory actions on spinal cord afferent projection neurons. Outputs from forebrain regions, including the ACC, PFC and amygdala reach the PAG, suggesting that these descending systems could be activated by psychological factors. Opioids are particularly powerful analgesics, and their analgesic action works at multiple sites within these descending pathways, including the ACC, PAG, RVM and dorsal horn of the spinal cord39,75.

Several studies that involved distraction from pain but used distraction procedures that probably increased arousal and/or negative emotions showed activation of this fronto–PAG–brainstem circuit67,76,77,78. However, when emotion and arousal were dissociated from attention4, it was the increased negative emotion that was associated with activation of the ACC–fronto–PAG circuitry. Furthermore, this activation correlated with negative mood-related increases in pain-evoked activity in the ACC (Fig. 3). By contrast, when subjects intentionally directed their attention to or away from a painful stimulus, attention-related alterations in pain-evoked activity in the insula correlated with activity in the superior parietal cortex (Brodmann area 7 (BA7)). This region is part of Corbetta and Shulman's proposed 'top-down orienting of attention' system, which for visual attention involves the frontal eye fields and the superior parietal cortex79. Parts of BA7 project to the S1, S2 and insula80,81,82, providing a direct corticocortical pathway for the attentional modulation of pain, and descending pathways from the insula to the amygdala provide a possible descending component to the attentional modulation of pain14 (Fig. 3). Note that in addition to a top-down attention system, there is a bottom-up, stimulus-driven attentional circuit that is specialized in the detection of salient or unexpected stimuli79, involving the temporoparietal cortex and inferior frontal cortex (which is largely lateralized to the right hemisphere). This circuit could be differentially engaged when pain is presented in different emotional contexts.

Anticipation of pain relief activates descending pathways. Anticipation of the relief of pain is a primary contributor to placebo analgesia2. Brain imaging studies of expectation-related placebo analgesia find that immediately preceding the presentation of a noxious stimulus, when an individual receives a placebo 'analgesic' and is expecting a reduction in pain, there is activation of a ACC–fronto–PAG pathway83,84. Interestingly, this involves similar brain regions as those that are activated when a positive emotional state reduces pain. Moreover, placebo treatment alters endogenous opioid activity in parts of this pathway, including the PAG, ACC and PFC85. Further evidence that this ACC–fronto–PAG pathway is involved in placebo analgesia comes from findings showing that naloxone (a μ-opioid antagonist) modulates placebo-induced responses in the PAG and RVM and abolishes the coupling between rostral ACC and PAG activity83. There is also behavioural evidence that naloxone reduces expectation-related placebo analgesia in humans86 and conditioned placebo-analgesia in rodents87,88. Thus, it appears that descending modulatory pathways involved in placebo analgesia may overlap with those involved in the emotional modulation of pain. Conversely, circuitry involved in the attentional modulation of pain, including the superior parietal cortex and the insula, does not appear to be activated during expectation-related placebo analgesia. This independence of placebo analgesia from the attentional modulation of pain was further confirmed in a study showing additive reductions in pain caused by placebo and distraction89, indicating that placebo analgesia does not depend on the active redirection of attention. The finding that emotional factors, anticipation and attention alter pain processing is important in the therapeutic context. A patient receiving pharmacotherapy may have an enhanced or diminished response depending on his expectations, emotional state or focus of attention.

Effects of chronic pain on pain modulation

Patients with chronic pain often report that stimuli that should be innocuous are in fact painful for them. This can range from normal joint movement causing pain in patients with arthritis, to the touch of clothes against the skin causing burning pain in patients with nerve injuries. In laboratory studies, patients with various chronic pain syndromes, including arthritis, back pain, fibromyalgia, irritable bowel syndrome (IBS) and vulvar vestibulitis, show higher pain ratings and enhanced pain-evoked neural responses when experimental pain stimuli are presented than controls90,91,92,93,94,95. Even stimuli that are not painful in healthy people but are perceived to be painful by patients produce a pain-related activation pattern in the brain91,92,94.

The increased activation of pain pathways may arise from peripheral and/or central sensitization in ascending nociceptive pathways, but there is evidence that some of the amplification may arise from abnormalities in descending modulatory systems75,96. Perceptual evidence for abnormalities in descending pain modulation in patients with chronic pain comes from studies of conditioned pain modulation. In these paradigms, a painful test stimulus is evaluated in the absence and presence of a second painful (conditioning) stimulus applied to a remote body region. In a normally functioning nociceptive system, the amount of pain experienced at the primary testing site will be reduced when the second noxious stimulus is presented (classically termed counter-irritation or 'pain inhibits pain'). Studies in anaesthetized rodents show a spino–brainstem–spinal modulatory loop that reduces afferent nociceptive activation, which is termed diffuse noxious inhibitory control97. However, in conscious humans, supraspinal mechanisms also contribute to conditioned pain modulation98. More than 30 studies have now examined conditioned pain modulation in patients with chronic pain, with varying paradigms and mixed results. Nevertheless, a recent meta-analysis concluded that conditioned pain modulation is impaired in populations with chronic pain99. This impairment in a system that normally reduces pain could contribute to the enhanced pain perception observed in patients with chronic pain. Functional brain imaging studies in patients with chronic pain also support the idea that endogenous pain modulatory systems may be dysfunctional in these patients. In response to experimental pain stimuli, patients with chronic pain disorders, including fibromyalgia, IBS and back pain, show abnormal activation patterns in brain regions involved in pain regulation, including the rostral ACC and frontal cortex100,101,102,103.

Structural changes in pain modulatory systems in chronic pain. There is now a wealth of evidence suggesting that patients with chronic pain may have anatomical alterations within regions involved in cognitive and emotional modulation of pain, such as the dorsolateral and medial PFC, the ACC and the insula. For example, there is less grey matter in the brains of patients with chronic back pain than in the brains of age-matched control subjects, especially in the dorsolateral PFC104,105,106. Grey matter loss has also been reported in patients suffering from other chronic pain disorders, such as fibromyalgia, headache, IBS, complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) and osteoarthritis (for a review, see Ref. 107). Although grey matter reductions are found in multiple brain regions, the most common regions to manifest such reductions are the insula, ACC and PFC. In addition to grey matter changes, alterations in white matter are beginning to be revealed using diffusion-weighted imaging techniques such as diffusion tensor imaging, in which fractional anisotropy — a measure of directionality of water movement — is measured as a surrogate for white matter integrity. These studies suggest that various types of chronic pain can lead to disruptions in white matter tracts108,109,110,111,112,113,114. Although conventional MRI cannot determine the histopathology underlying grey and white matter changes, several lines of evidence suggest the possibility that excessive nociceptive input may impair grey matter structure and function, including possible neuronal loss related to excitotoxicity115. Studies in rats and mice suggest that a chronic pain state can cause a supraspinal neuroinflammatory response116,117 in addition to changes in dendritic and synaptic structure and function in regions involved in pain processing118,119. Together with the anatomical data, this suggests that patients with chronic pain may undergo degenerative processes or at least functional alterations within brain areas involved in cognitively modulated analgesia.

The neurochemical basis of cognitive and emotional modulation of pain. Studies are now beginning to show that patients with chronic pain may have altered neurochemistry in brain systems that are involved in psychologically modulated analgesia. Findings from studies using in vivo proton magnetic resonance spectrometry show increases in glutamate and/or decreases of the neuronal marker N-acetyl aspartate in frontal cortices of patients with chronic back pain and fibromyalgia120,121,122,123,124; such findings support the idea that reduced grey matter in patients with chronic pain may be related to possible excitotoxicity. One study using transcranial magnetic stimulation found lower intracortical facilitation and shorter intracortical inhibition in patients with fibromyalgia than in controls125, suggesting that there are deficits in intracortical modulation involving both GABAergic and glutamatergic mechanisms. Other studies showed changes in opioidergic and dopaminergic systems in patients with chronic pain and rodent models of chronic pain. Several molecular imaging studies using positron emission tomography showed cerebral decreases in opioid receptor binding in patients with neuropathic pain, rheumatoid arthritis and fibromyalgia126,127,128,129. Decreases in the levels of dopamine in the forebrain of patients with fibromyalgia have also been reported130. Opioidergic dysfunction was also seen in the cortex and amygdala of mice with chronic inflammation or nerve injury, which was related to increases in anxiety-like behaviours131,132. Taken together, these studies suggest that changes in neurotransmitter systems may mean that patients with chronic pain have decreased receptor availability or heightened endogenous release of these neurotransmitters. In either case it seems that neurochemicals that are important for placebo analgesia are not acting in the same way as they do in healthy people.

Together, the evidence of altered neuroanatomical and neurochemical functioning in brain regions involved in the cognitive modulation of pain leads to the prediction that patients with chronic pain should have reduced or altered cognitive modulation of pain compared with healthy individuals (Fig. 4).

Figure 4: Consistently identified changes in the brains of patients with chronic pain.
figure 4

The three cortical regions that consistently show decreases in grey matter are the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), prefrontal cortex (PFC) and insula. Studies have also identified changes in white matter integrity in these regions; such changes are manifested by decreased fractional anisotropy (FA), which suggests that there is a decrease in white matter health. Molecular imaging studies show decreases in opioid receptor binding in patients with chronic pain in all three regions. Studies using in vivo proton magnetic resonance spectrometry show chronic pain-related decreases of the neuronal marker N-acetyl aspartate (NAA) in the frontal cortex and the insula. Finally, rodent studies show increased neuroinflammation in the ACC and PFC. Black arrows show the descending pathways from Fig. 3.Grey arrows show afferent pain pathways.

PowerPoint slide

Is there evidence that disruption of endogenous pain modulatory systems alters cognitive and emotional modulation of pain in patients with chronic pain? There is now accumulating evidence that the impairments observed in patients with chronic pain go beyond the pain itself. These patients also show changes across cognitive and emotional domains, and similar changes are found in preclinical models of chronic pain133 (Box 3). For example, patients with fibromyalgia are less able to retain new information when rehearsal is prevented by a distraction than healthy controls134,135,136. Patients with back pain, fibromyalgia or CRPS have all been shown to have deficits in emotional decision-making when they were tested with the Iowa gambling task137,138,139. Preclinical studies confirm deficits in emotional decision-making tasks in rodent chronic pain models140. Deficits in these tasks are also observed in patients with disorders such as traumatic brain injury and multiple sclerosis who have lesions in brain regions implicated in descending pain modulation, including the medial PFC and rostral ACC141,142,143,144,145. Furthermore, the brain regions that are altered in patients with chronic pain are among those that are activated during the tasks in which patients perform poorly146,147. Thus, the anatomical changes observed in patients with chronic pain may well be an underlying factor in their altered performance in cognitive tasks.

If the chronic pain-related anatomical alterations in brain regions involved in cognition, emotions and pain modulation truly have a functional consequence, then one would predict that patients with chronic pain should have altered psychological modulation of pain, including possible alterations in placebo responsiveness. Alternatively, the patients could have normal psychological modulation of pain, but they may be using different brain circuitry to achieve the modulation. Few studies have tested this concept, but there is some evidence pointing to both decreased psychological modulation of pain and the involvement of novel pathways in such modulation. For example, in response to relaxation, patients with IBS demonstrate reduced modulation of rectal distension-induced pain and reduced activation of the insula148. Conversely, these patients demonstrate more pronounced stress-induced modulation of neural activation in multiple brain sites involved in pain modulation, including the insula and ventrolateral PFC148. Whereas some studies find similar attentional149 and affective150 modulation of pain between patients with fibromyalgia and healthy controls, other studies have observed deficits in attentional pain modulation in patients with chronic pain151 and abnormal affective modulation of somatosensory brain processing in patients with fibromyalgia152. In terms of placebo analgesia, a study conducted in patients with IBS (a type of chronic visceral pain) found a placebo-related perceptual pain reduction but observed, contrary to findings in healthy subjects, that the opioid receptor antagonist naloxone did not reduce the placebo response, suggesting that any placebo response shown in patients with IBS was not opioid-mediated153. Although accumulating evidence suggests that patients with chronic pain may have altered psychological modulation of pain and altered modulatory systems in the brain, more direct comparisons between patients with chronic pain and healthy subjects need to be made to fully understand how these modulatory systems may be affected by the continued presence of pain.

Can altered pain modulatory circuitry be reversed in patients with chronic pain? Several studies reveal that when a painful condition, such as hip osteoarthritis, headache or back pain, is eliminated after an individual has suffered chronic pain for years, grey matter reductions can be reversed. In these studies, brain regions implicated in pain modulation, including the dorsolateral PFC and ACC, had reduced grey matter in patients, but after successful treatment for the pain, the grey matter reductions were reversed so that the affected brain regions normalized in size104,154,155,156. These findings suggest that grey matter decreases related to chronic pain are not necessarily caused by neuronal death, but instead by other changes in neuronal tissue, such as reduced dendritic or synaptic density and possible changes in non-neuronal tissue119,157,158. Nevertheless, the anatomical normalization of brain regions involved in the psychological modulation of pain corresponds with chronic pain reductions after surgical or pharmacological treatment.

If cognitive and emotional factors can intrinsically activate modulatory circuitry in brain regions showing anatomical changes due to chronic pain, can psychological treatments reverse brain changes associated with chronic pain? Although there is little research available to date, some evidence suggests that answer may be 'yes'. Cognitive behavioural therapy has been shown to increase pain-evoked neural activation in the dorsolateral and ventrolateral PFC: that is, in regions involved in pain modulation159. Meditation, alternatively, reduces pain-evoked PFC activity160,161 but increases activity in the rostral ACC and the anterior insula: that is, in areas involved in the cognitive regulation of nociceptive processing162. Although there is no direct evidence that activation of the pain regulatory circuitry has long-term functional consequences, there are a few studies showing that people who meditate have thicker cortices in frontal regions, including the PFC, ACC and insula8,163,164,164. Studies have not yet addressed the impact of such mind–body therapies on the brains of patients with chronic pain, but current evidence suggests that they may have a neuroprotective effect.

Conclusions, implications and limitations

There are huge inter-individual as well as intra-individual differences in pain perception depending on the context and meaning of the pain. Evidence presented in this Review shows that much of this variation can be explained by the interplay between afferent nociceptive signals to the brain and descending modulatory systems that are activated endogenously by cognitive and emotional factors. The descending modulatory systems involve brain regions that are important not only for pain but also for cognitive and emotional functioning in general166. Thus, alterations in the anatomical integrity and functioning of brain regions involved in both pain control and cognitive and/or emotional functioning could explain why patients with long-term chronic pain develop cognitive deficits as well as anxiety disorders and depression. Whereas the temporal relationship between pain, cognitive deficits, anxiety and depression is difficult to determine in most patients with pain, longitudinal animal studies now suggest that emotional and cognitive alterations may sometimes begin long after the onset of pain. For example, in a rodent model of neuropathic pain, rats exhibited anxiety-like behaviour and attentional deficits months after the injury and the onset of hypersensitivity, which was temporally coincident with anatomical changes in the frontal cortex167,168. Thus, there is growing evidence that pain may be detrimental to the brain and that long-term pain itself can both decrease an individual's ability to endogenously control the pain and lead to many of the comorbidities that plague individuals with chronic pain. The old adage 'no pain, no gain' should probably be discarded in favour of 'no pain, healthy brain'.

Nevertheless, caution is required when interpreting the results of the studies cited in this Review, as many of them involve small sample sizes and do not always include comprehensive control measures. Furthermore, most of the brain imaging studies are cross-sectional, so causal relationships are difficult to ascertain. Thus, longitudinal studies involving larger groups, as well as more preclinical studies back-translating clinical observations, will be important to fully confirm the hypothesis that chronic pain can alter pain modulatory systems in the brain.