Main

Many people experience voluntary or involuntary career breaks at some point during their working lives1, leading to employment gaps on their résumés. Such employment gaps may be caused by external shocks (for example, sickness or downsizing due to the COVID-19 pandemic2,3) or career and lifestyle choices. Women are particularly affected by employment gaps when they take family-related leaves; for example, in the United Kingdom, over 70% of previously full-time working women take between 6 and 18 months out of paid employment after the birth of a child4.

Even when employment gaps are transitory, workers may face discrimination upon work re-entry if these gaps are evident on their résumés5,6,7,8,9,10. Whereas traditionally structural unemployment (for example, skill shortages) is a major concern to the economy and society at large11, frictional unemployment (for example, short gaps between jobs or short-term leave) may pose challenges for individuals—in particular, the scarring effects of short-term unemployment gaps12. Indeed, this underemployment in itself is a problem due to inefficiency, but could also lead to more structural problems if those job seekers decide to leave the labour market permanently. While penalties associated with employment gaps have been shown to affect male and female workers6,7,13, motherhood penalties may particularly penalize women for childcare-related leaves5. There is a long literature noting the scarring effects of gaps in employment and a closely aligned literature exploring the impact of maternity leave and adjacent career breaks on individuals’ career trajectories12. In fact, these additional barriers to re-entry for mothers may contribute to the well-known, persistent gender wage gap12,14,15,16 as well as to women’s lower representation in the upper echelons of companies17,18,19,20. These effects are likely compounded further by other factors that also contribute to gender inequalities in the labour market, including occupational segregation and differential job entry21,22, hiring agencies’ pre-emptive sorting by gender and industries23, bias and discrimination within the workplace24, negotiation decisions25 and differential career advancement26.

In this article, we focus on an early stage of the process: the initial screening of résumés—the first ‘gateway’—when companies hire for a new position. To study discrimination during the hiring process against workers with employment gaps, researchers have in the past turned to audit studies. Audit studies27 have been used extensively to examine the effects of gender (such as discrimination against women28,29), race (such as discrimination against non-whites30), unemployment (that is, discrimination against those who are unemployed)6,7,31,32, and more recently, parenthood and childcare-related leave (that is, discrimination against parents taking time out of the workforce to care for their children5,10,33); for a comprehensive register of discrimination of various characteristics during hiring in audit experiments, see Baert27. These studies measure the effects of applicant characteristics on ‘callbacks’ (that is, an employer invitation to the next stage in the recruitment process—often a job interview).

While reduced opportunities for workers with employment gaps have been widely documented, little research has explored ways to overcome these barriers and biases. Some research has focused on reducing bias towards female applicants and working mothers. Of these interventions, employer strategies require manager training34,35, suppressing biases and taking more time to review applicants36, or overhauling current assessment processes37. Employee strategies encourage applicants to explain their employment gaps8,38,39, highlight volunteer work40 or deliberately manage others’ impressions28,41. Although these interventions have shown promise, they also tend to require substantial extra effort from applicants and employers; some of these strategies may even create backlash or social penalties by creating incongruence with behavioural expectations for women41.

To reduce these burdens, we develop and test a costless intervention applicants can adopt to facilitate workforce re-entry without backlash. Our intervention is informed by research from psychology and the field of judgement and decision-making, which shows that people inherently categorize people into groups, particularly when the category is easily accessible and representative42,43. Stereotype activation is an automatic process, but reliance on these stereotypes is also greater in contexts of high uncertainty and high subjectivity44,45,46,47, which characterizes many personnel selection processes48. In addition to reliance on stereotypes (for example, mothers are less committed to their jobs and less productive than their child-free and male counterparts, unemployed applicants are lower quality and less productive than employed persons, etc.), employers may also be comparing applicants to prototypical workers.

We therefore hypothesize that employees with employment gaps contrast with conceptions of the ‘ideal worker’ who begins employment in early adulthood, continuing full-time without interruption for several decades49. Whether it is a mother who has taken a caregiving leave or a person who became unemployed due to job loss —the two most common reasons for disrupted employment—career breaks undermine decision-makers’ impressions of applicant job experience by breaking this pattern of continuous employment50,51.

Employers may still attend to career breaks (and may even discount previous work experience) despite the break’s potential irrelevance for the quality of the worker; we therefore argue that it is desirable to obscure this information from decision-makers. Our intervention removes the career-break information from job-seekers’ résumés, while still conveying job-relevant information. Specifically, to decrease the salience of the employment gap and to increase the salience of applicant experience, our intervention displays work experience in a different format: the number of years of experience for each job held (Supplementary Fig. 1b) instead of the standard ‘date format’ (Supplementary Fig. 1a). That is, instead of an applicant’s résumé listing the two calendar dates between which the applicant started and finished a job (for example, ‘March 2011–March 2016’), the treatment résumé displays a single number indicating the number of years the applicant worked in each job (for example, ‘5 years’). As a result, the intervention draws attention to the applicants’ job experience while also obfuscating employment gaps by omission.

We hypothesize that our intervention will increase the likelihood of a qualified applicant advancing to the next stage of selection (such as receiving a callback in Study 1, or receiving increased ratings of perceived hireability in Studies 2–3). To test our theorized mechanism—perceived job experience—we also measure recalled years of experience (Studies 2–3). A related but separate research question that we do not address here is whether applicants in the treatment group are treated differently from the control group once they progress past the first gateway (for example, at an in-person interview). While unequal treatment can still occur at the interview stage52,53,54, other research aims to reduce bias during this stage of the application process55,56. The powerful, lasting effects of first impressions and the necessity of passing the first gateway to get to the second gateway57 further underlines the importance of the current research.

Given the high prevalence of employment gaps among women due to family-related leave—which also remains a critical contributor to workplace gender inequalities—a key focus of our studies is on mothers returning to work. Studying discrimination against mothers (and fathers) has been a particular focus in the literature. Notably, Correll and colleagues found evidence of discrimination against mothers who received half as many callbacks as child-free women but no callback penalties for fathers (versus child-free men)5. Weisshaar10 found no statistically significant gender differences in callbacks. However, employed parents (versus unemployed parents who were laid off) received approximately 1.8 times more callbacks, and were approximately three times more likely to get a callback (versus parents who voluntarily left to take care of their children)10. Although our later studies also include men, this was primarily intended to test potential boundary conditions of our intervention. However, results from these additional studies show that the intervention appears to be useful for a range of job seekers: for men and women with various reasons for employment gaps and lengths of job experience.

Results

In a real-world setting with actual employers, Study 1 revealed that displaying the number of years of job experience (Years condition) on a résumé garnered more callbacks for job-seeking mothers than any other condition (Fig. 1). The other conditions are No Gap, where the résumé had the most recent employment date running from ‘July 2015 to Present’; an Unexplained Gap condition, where the last date in employment ended 2.5 years before the résumé was sent out, and an Explained Gap condition, where the last date in employment ended 2.5 years before the résumé was sent out, followed by the sentence, ‘Left to become a full-time mother and look after my children’.

Fig. 1: Callback rates by condition.
figure 1

The graph shows the percentage of callbacks per condition. Using a linear probability model, we find that the Years condition (n = 2,255) received significantly more callbacks than the No Gap condition (n = 2,255), the Explained Gap condition (n = 2,256) and the Unexplained Gap condition (n = 2,256). The error bars represent standard errors from the mean. The Unexplained Gap (b = −0.049, s.e. = 0.014, t(9,003) = −3.52, P < 0.001; d = −0.10, 95% CI −0.18 to −0.02) and Explained Gap (b = −0.050, s.e. = 0.014, t(9,003) = −3.61, P < 0.001; d = −0.10, 95% CI −0.18 to −0.02) conditions led to significantly lower callbacks than the Years condition. Furthermore, even the No Gap condition, which served as a conservative benchmark, received fewer callbacks (b = −0.029, s.e. = 0.014, t(9,003) = −2.07, P = 0.038, d = −0.06, 95%CI −0.14 to 0.02) than the Years condition. These results can also be found in Supplementary Table 2A.

Using linear probability models controlling for working pattern and region (as described in the preregistration), both the Unexplained Gap (b = −0.049, standard error (s.e.) = 0.014, t(9,003) = −3.52, P < 0.001; effect size (d) = −0.10, 95% confidence interval (CI) −0.18 to −0.02) and Explained Gap (b = −0.050, s.e = 0.014, t(9,003) = −3.61, P < 0.001; d = −0.10, 95% CI −0.18 to −0.02) conditions led to significantly lower callbacks than the Years condition. Furthermore, even the No Gap condition, which served as a conservative benchmark, received fewer callbacks (b = −0.029, s.e = 0.014, t(9,003) = −2.07, P = 0.038; d = −0.06, 95% CI −0.14 to 0.02) than the Years condition. All results hold when including job types and county fixed effects, as well as when using a logistic regression model (Supplementary Table 2b). In sum, and as predicted, the redesigned résumé improved job prospects for mothers returning to paid employment in a large-scale field experiment, even when compared to similar mothers without employment gaps.

Our first study offers evidence that the Years intervention led to more callbacks for applicants in a real-world setting with real employers. To better understand the mechanism through which the Years résumé operates, we turned to controlled online vignette studies58. We were particularly interested in capturing how the Years intervention is perceived along a number of dimensions (measured through Likert scales, Methods) in contrast to the standard résumé, although we also sought to capture a hypothetical proxy for our outcome variable (callback) in the field study. We used a ‘hireability’ outcome59, measured on a scale from 0 to 100, which captured the likelihood that the study participant would advance the applicant to the next stage of the application process.

We first explored the possible mechanisms with an online pilot study, in which we found no evidence in support of the most parsimonious explanations, namely, that the Years treatment is seen as easier to read (b = 0.08, s.e. = 0.15, t(248) = 0.51, P = 0.61; d = 0.01, 95% CI −0.34 to 0.36) or more novel (b = 0.01, s.e. = 0.16, t(248) = 0.06, P = 0.95; d = 0.06, 95% CI −0.30 to 0.41). Suggestive evidence for the mechanism emerged as increased perceptions of overall applicant experience in the treatment (b = 0.37, s.e. = 0.12, t(248) = 3.19, P = 0.002; d = 0.40, 95% CI 0.04 to 0.76) and years of applicant experience that participants recalled (b = 0.59, s.e. = 0.29, t(248) = 2.00, P = 0.047; d = 0.25, 95% CI −0.11 to 0.61). For the full regression results, see Supplementary Table 3.

Our preregistered Study 2 aimed to test this mechanism of increased perceptions of experience more explicitly and with a larger sample (n = 800). Study 2 was similar in many ways to Study 1 but differed from it in that we expanded it to also include résumés from male applicants. In particular, because the intervention in Study 1 was successful for applicants without an employment gap, we also sought to test whether the intervention would be moderated by, or would interact with, applicant gender.

Study 2 replicated and extended the effect of the Years condition, demonstrating that there was no statistically significant moderation by applicant gender: the redesigned résumé led applicants to be evaluated as more likely to be hired than applicants using a standard résumé, both when controlling for applicant gender (treatment: b = 2.13, s.e. = 0.94, t(758) = 2.25, P = 0.025; d = 0.16, 95% CI −0.04 to 0.36; applicant gender: b = −1.31, s.e. = 0.94, t(758) = −1.39, P = 0.17; d = 0.10, 95% CI −0.30 to 0.10; see Supplementary Table 4a, columns 1 and 2) and when including an interaction term between applicant gender and the intervention (treatment: b = 3.29, s.e. = 1.33, t(757) = 2.47, P = 0.01; gender: b = −0.12, s.e. = 1.35, t(757) = −0.09, P = 0.93; and treatment × gender interaction: b = −2.34, s.e. = 1.89, t(757) = −1.24, P = 0.22; see Supplementary Table 4a, column 3). Furthermore, Supplementary Table 4a, column 4 shows the robustness of the results by including both the interaction term and job fixed effects, while column 5 shows robustness by additionally excluding participants whose responses were outliers in the top 1% for the variable of years recalled.

We also confirmed the role of years of experience as a key mechanism: while the actual amount of job experience was 10 cumulative years for applicants in both conditions, participants who evaluated a résumé in the Years treatment more accurately recalled the number of years of experience that the applicant had (mean (M ) = 9.41, s.e. = 0.34) than those in the standard résumé condition (M = 8.35, s.e. = 0.24; b = 1.06, t(759) = 3.16, P = 0.002; d = 0.23, 95% CI 0.03 to 0.43). This finding held after controlling for applicant gender and job type (Supplementary Table 4b) and was not significantly moderated by either or both factors.

In our preregistration, we said that we would exclude those who failed the gender manipulation check because we figured that those individuals would not be paying sufficient attention to the task at hand. For robustness, we provide the intention-to-treat (ITT) analysis without those exclusions; however, we expect adding in these additional inattentive participants would introduce noise to our analysis. In the ITT analysis, the treatment effect on applicant advancement becomes slightly marginal in two specifications (Supplementary Table 4c: P = 0.054 in our baseline specification with the treatment dummy in column 1; and P = 0.056 with job fixed effects in column 2) and remains significant in the two remaining specifications (Supplementary Table 4c: P = 0.021 when we include the interaction term in column 3; and P = 0.021 when we include both the interaction term and job fixed effects in column 4). Furthermore, in the ITT analysis, the treatment effect on recalled years of applicant experience is significant across all specifications (Supplementary Table 4d). In sum, the findings from the robustness analyses are broadly consistent with our preregistered analyses, although the estimates in some specifications are noisier, which we discuss in more detail below.

Finally, we sought to explore a policy-relevant boundary condition of the intervention. As the Years intervention focuses hiring managers’ attention on applicants’ amount of accumulated experience, it is plausible that the effect becomes less pronounced for more experienced workers (whose prior experience may be sufficiently long to be imprinted on hiring managers even with the standard résumé) or for less experienced workers (whose prior experience is too short to be highlighted effectively with the Years intervention).

Our preregistered Study 3 (n = 1,600) demonstrated that neither of these potential boundary conditions is of particular concern: the Years intervention worked successfully for applicants with 5 years or 15 years of experience, increasing hireability for applicants with fewer years of experience (5 years) (b = 2.36, s.e. = 1.03, t(762) = 2.29, P = 0.023; d = 0.17, 95% CI −0.02 to 0.32, Supplementary Table 5a, column 1) and with a greater number of years of experience (15 years) (b = 2.21, s.e. = 0.99, t(755) = 2.23, P = 0.026; d = 0.16, 95% CI 0.02 to 0.29, Supplementary Table 5a, column 2). In our preregistration, we said that we would exclude those who failed the gender manipulation check; however, for robustness, we include the ITT analysis without those exclusions (Supplementary Table 5b). All results remain significant except the probability of applicant advancement for 15 years of experience, which is marginal (P = 0.052). In sum, the findings from these robustness analyses are broadly consistent with our preregistered analyses.

Discussion

While the onus should not be on unemployed applicants to prevent others’ bias against them, ample evidence has demonstrated that applicants with employment gaps face lower employment prospects, and therefore would benefit from seeking ways to remain competitive when re-entering the workforce. For working mothers in particular, a frequently recommended strategy is to ‘explain the gap’39. Despite this proactive attempt to reframe the conversation—highlighting the skills, dedication and hard work needed to be a caregiver—we found no empirical support that this strategy works any better than an unexplained gap in our large-scale audit experiment in Study 1 (Supplementary Table 2c). However, our results from the field experiment offer applicants a promising and effective strategy to overcome barriers to work re-entry. Low-effort and costless, our intervention replaces the standard employment dates on the résumé with the length of time of employment and thereby highlights applicants’ experience to prospective employers, eliminating employment gap penalties that hinder these applicants’ advancement beyond the first gateway of the selection process. Furthermore, by conducting this study in a field setting, we prioritize high external validity. However, in a field setting it can be more difficult (and more expensive) to test mechanism and boundary conditions. Therefore, we combined these findings with additional studies in a more controlled ‘online lab’ setting for Studies 2 and 3 (ref. 60).

Given the positive callback outcomes of the redesigned résumé for women in Study 1 compared to both No Gap and Gap résumés, we expanded this research to also include male applicants. In an online study, we tested and found that the intervention works well; also, its success is not moderated by the gender of the applicant, even when compared to résumés without an employment gap. These results suggest that résumés could be improved for a variety of applicants. And while there was no evidence that the treatment had an effect on perceptions of novelty or ease of reading, Study 2 demonstrated that the redesigned résumés facilitated reviewers’ recall of applicants’ years of job experience. Our final Study 3 provided additional evidence that this treatment can work for applicants with shorter and longer job experience, further suggesting that this intervention is fairly generalizable for various types of applicants. Because findings from our field studies and online vignette studies converge, we believe this is promising for the validity of our results60.

Our research makes several contributions. First, this intervention provides a blueprint for how the judgement and decision-making literature can theoretically and practically contribute to practical interventions in the real world: by taking into account the mental machinery of hiring managers, we show how the kinds of mental shortcuts that can lead to bias (for example, seeing only gaps in employment) can instead be redirected to focus on positive associations (for example, helping hiring managers appreciate applicants’ accumulated experience). Our research further contributes to the literature on gender discrimination, demonstrating a costless way for returning working mothers to show their potential to hiring managers and have a chance to proceed past the first gateway. Finally, our research contributes to understanding the wider experiences of discrimination for men and women who were temporarily unemployed. Helping people return to work after a prolonged unemployment spell is critical for public policy and social welfare support processes.

While this intervention predicted more callbacks and greater hireability, it is possible that this progress could be undone later in the interview process. For example, hiring managers might enquire about the exact dates of employment during an interview and, if learning about an employment gap, treat these applicants more negatively. However, it is also possible that interviewers rely less on stereotypes at this later stage, thus granting applicants a fairer, more merit-based opportunity. We encourage future research to explore this possibility. Furthermore, as hiring managers seem to assume that applicants with the standard ‘dates’ résumé have less experience than those with the ‘years’ résumé, future research should also attempt to quantify exactly how many years of experience the intervention can compensate for.

Our studies necessarily involved several design choices that other researchers may choose to explore differently. First, we focused on between-subject designs for our studies. While both between-subject and within-subject designs have their respective strengths and weaknesses, by not exposing participants to both treatment and control sequentially, the between-subject design is often a ‘cleaner’ if statistically less efficient test of causality61,62. On the other hand, we cannot speak to whether the same decision-maker would make different choices between the two résumés, which we encourage future research to explore. An additional consideration for choosing the between-subject design in the field context was that it reduces the burden on each individual employer (that is, the same employer is not sent multiple fictitious résumés). Second, we chose to replicate our field findings using online subject samples. While moving from the field into the ‘online lab’ reduces external validity, it also offers more experimental control and the potential to explore underlying mechanisms (for example, via survey scales)63. We chose to run our studies on Prolific Academic because it enabled us to reach a sample of working adults in the United Kingdom, which was similar to our field experiment sample64. Additionally, recent research on data quality across multiple platforms has shown Prolific to be of substantially higher quality than alternative platforms65. Because our results converge in both the field and online settings, it heightens our confidence in these findings.

However, there are also several limitations of this work. First, we only tested this intervention in the United Kingdom; however, we believe these findings should generalize because of the mechanism we identified. The ‘years’ résumé seems to operate on a cognitive level, not a cultural level. Therefore, we would expect this intervention to be effective in countries with less generous parental leave policies (for example, the United States) or more generous policies (for example, Scandinavian countries). That said, we encourage researchers to experimentally test the effectiveness of the intervention in other countries. Furthermore, as we only tested four specific levels of job experience (that is, 5, 9, 10 and 15 years), it is possible that there may be a lower bound of experience (for example, ≤1 year) below which the ‘years’ résumé might actually make a résumé appear less impressive than the standard ‘dates’ résumé. We also believe that the positive effects of this intervention may be limited to fields where more years is a proxy for more experience, and thus viewed favourably. If, however, a job applicant had a career break in certain fields (for example, while finishing a PhD in an academic context), the ‘years’ résumé might call attention to the extended timeframe, potentially triggering a negative effect (for example, signalling low motivation)13. Another potential limitation is in Studies 2 and 3 where we preregistered our analysis to exclude participants who did not pay sufficient attention and failed the attention check in the study. Doing so reduces the extent to which our results allow for a causal interpretation for all participants; rather they represent the causal treatment effect for participants who paid attention (that is, treatment-on-treated). However, our results are largely robust—with two out of eight regression specifications becoming marginally significant and the other six specifications remaining significant—to including even participants who did not pay sufficient attention in the study. Finally, a potential limitation of our design in Study 1 is that both the CV and the cover letter changed, introducing a potential confound. While this means that we cannot precisely identify which element in Study 1 caused our main effect, there is additional evidence that is consistent with our conclusion about the ‘years’ résumé: we replicated the main effect in online studies, where we only manipulated the résumés and did not provide a cover letter.

Our audit study was primarily conducted before the onset of COVID-19, yet it might offer insights into how employees can navigate a pandemic-induced employment gap. Due to the COVID-19 pandemic, millions of women and men now have employment gaps on their résumés66, especially previously working mothers3. While hiring penalties may be lower for applicants whose employment gaps are due to external forces10, the intervention tested here could theoretically help all applicants to receive appropriate recognition for their years of job experience.

While our results primarily speak to applicants, we believe this research also contributes to understanding ways stereotyping can be overcome and helping organizations with the design of their hiring processes. Hiring managers can add this intervention to their toolbox of ‘debiasing’ strategies (that is, by explicitly requesting that all résumés be submitted with years instead of dates), just as ‘blinding’ résumés has become commonplace in many settings24. While the general equilibrium effect of this intervention is an important question for future research if this intervention becomes more widely adopted, we predict that it would generally contribute to levelling the playing field if adopted more widely across applicants with and without employment gaps. In this way, applicants with equal experience receive equal employment opportunities, without the biasing stereotypes that more salient gaps may evoke.

Methods

Materials, data and code for all studies are available at https://osf.io/3gahc. Ethics oversight for the field experiment was provided by the Behavioural Insights Team’s internal ethics process, and ethics oversight for the online lab experiments was provided by the University of Exeter ethics committee (eUEBS003871) and the Harvard University IRB (IRB20-1467). It is worth noting that the initial field study (Study 1) did not obtain explicit informed consent due to the impossibility of mitigating deception in this design; there was also no debriefing, which the research team deemed would create more harm than benefit. Moreover, the email inboxes and phones were monitored daily, and the research team politely declined any positive callbacks within one working day to reduce the potential burden on employers. Participants in the online studies did provide informed consent.

Study 1

We aimed to send 9,000 applications to detect an effect size of d = 0.08 with 80% power. We manipulated the presentation of the applicant’s prior experience in a job in the form of dates (as is the case on traditional résumés) or summarizing the number of years the applicant held the job (on the redesigned résumé). We sent one of four different résumés and cover letters (conditions described below) to 9,022 employers across eight different sectors representing high- and low-skill jobs, in both male- and female-dominated fields (that is, software engineering, human resources, call centre operations, warehouse operations, finance, manufacturing production management, administrative work and social care work) who were advertising vacancies on a job-search platform from October 2019 to March 2020 in the United Kingdom. We aimed to assess a broad range of jobs that vary in the representation of men and women as well as the extent to which the job requirements might be linked to the male or female gender58.

All résumés belonged to a fictitious applicant who had 9 years of work experience, was employed in two previous roles and, most importantly, was a mother. We selected 9 years because the average age of women in the United Kingdom having their first child is 28.8 (ref. 67) and 50% of the population start full-time employment by 19 years old68, which implies approximately 9–10 years of work experience before the birth of a first child. The fictitious applicant was named ‘Sarah Smith’. Sarah was selected because it is one of the most common first names for women born in the United Kingdom between 1984 and 1994 (ref. 69) without strong associations with a particular social class70 and ‘Smith’ is the most common last name in the United Kingdom71. Where there was a gap, we selected a 2.5-year gap, because it is the average amount of time out of the workforce taken by women who choose to leave paid employment (beyond maternity or shared parental leave) for childcare-related reasons in the United Kingdom and then seek to return to paid employment72. We tailored the highest level of education and specifics of work experience to slightly exceed the typical requirements of each role. We conveyed parental status in all conditions with parent–teacher association involvement on résumés and stating that applicants were relocating to the hiring city with their family in cover letters5,10.

We randomly assigned employers to receive one of four résumés (and corresponding cover letters). Three conditions used the ‘traditional’ résumé format, listing previously held jobs with their corresponding dates of employment. We varied whether an employment gap was present and, if so, whether this gap was explained (by stating that the applicant took time out of the workforce to look after her children) or unexplained. In the No Gap condition, the résumé had the most recent employment date running from ‘July 2015 to Present’, along with a line in the cover letter that said, ‘I am currently employed at [Organization]’. In the Unexplained Gap condition, the last date in employment ended 2.5 years before the résumé was sent out and there was no explanation in the cover letter. In the Explained Gap condition, the last date in employment ended 2.5 years before the résumé was sent out, followed by the sentence, ‘Left to become a full-time mother and look after my children’. The Explained Gap condition also included the following sentence in the cover letter, ‘I was most recently employed at [Organization] and left in [Date] to become a full-time mother and care for my children, and am now eager to return to work’. We included the Explained Gap condition because it is a frequently recommended ‘solution’ on job-seekers’ websites and thus offers a useful comparison against a common real-world benchmark.

The fourth condition—the ‘Years’ condition—is our main treatment of interest, in which we replaced the dates of employment with the number of years in each role with no explicit mention of current employment in the cover letter. In this condition, employment gaps were, by design, not visible to the employer as this format conveys applicant job experience without revealing when the jobs were held.

We were interested in studying whether an application received a ‘callback’ from an employer. To capture callbacks, we assigned each condition a unique corresponding email address and phone number and monitored both. Following the literature5,10,29, we defined a callback as the employer progressing the applicant to the next stage in the process (for example, invitation to an online test, an interview or an in-person assessment), demonstrating strong positive interest, inquiring about start date availability, requesting that the applicant get in touch again once she moved, or if there was more than one missed call from the same employer. Our preregistration can be accessed at https://aspredicted.org/z2s6w.pdf.

The vast majority of applications for Study 1 (92.9%) were submitted before March 2020 when the United Kingdom enacted social distancing measures related to COVID-19. However, our results are also robust if we exclude data from March 2020 from the analysis.

Online pilot

For this exploratory study, we recruited 250 employees with hiring experience (33.6% male, Mage = 35.62, SDage = 12.38; see Supplementary information for details) from the United Kingdom through Prolific Academic and collected data using a Qualtrics survey. After being randomized to either the Traditional (no gap) or Years résumé of a female applicant, participants rated whether they found the resumé easy to read or novel and how much professional experience they thought the applicant had; participants also recalled the applicant’s years of job experience and demographics (for example, gender).

Study 2

We aimed to recruit 800 full-time employees from the United Kingdom through Prolific Academic and collected data using a Qualtrics survey to be able to detect an effect size of d = 0.20 with 80% power. After excluding participants who failed manipulation checks, we were left with 761 participants (54.7% male, Mage = 36.15, SDage = 10.68). We said we would exclude participants who failed the manipulation checks in our preregistration, so our main analysis here excludes them; however, we provide the full ITT analysis in the Supplementary information; these results are consistent with our main findings.

Participants saw one of two different job types (that is, software engineer, which is a traditionally male job, or human resources manager, which is a traditionally female job). Participants were then randomly assigned to view a male or a female applicant and a control (Traditional without a gap) or treatment (Years) résumé. After seeing the résumé, participants were asked, ‘How likely are you to advance this candidate to the next stage in the process?’ on a scale from 1 (Definitely not) to 100 (Definitely yes).

After seeing the résumé and rating the applicant, participants proceeded to the next page of the survey where they no longer saw the résumé and were asked to recall the number of years of experience the applicant had and the number of previous jobs the applicant held, as well as identify the gender of the applicant (a manipulation check). Our preregistration can be accessed at https://aspredicted.org/is2b7.pdf.

Study 3

We recruited participants residing in the United Kingdom through Prolific Academic and collected data using a Qualtrics survey. We aimed to recruit 1,600 participants to be able to detect an effect size of d = 0.25 with 80% power. We excluded participants who failed an attention check before randomization and those who failed the gender manipulation check. We were left with a sample of 1,521 participants (38.7% men, Mage = 34.8, SDage = 9.7). Because in our preregistration, we said that we would exclude these participants, our main analysis here excludes them; however, we provide the full ITT analysis in the Supplementary information; these results are consistent with our main findings.

Participants were randomly assigned to view the control (Traditional without a gap) or Years résumé. Within each condition, participants were then randomly assigned to see a résumé with fewer years (5 years) or more years (15 years) of job experience. Participants were then asked to rate on a 1–100 scale how likely they would be to advance the applicant to the next stage in the application process. After seeing the résumé and rating the applicant, participants proceeded to the next page of the survey where they no longer saw the résumé and were asked to recall the applicant’s number of years of job experience and their demographics (as in Study 2). Our preregistration can be accessed at https://aspredicted.org/id5m4.pdf.

Reporting summary

Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.