Introduction

Sensory processing is a physiological response to sensory events evoked by stimuli encountered in the environment1. For example, sensory processing provides social information by integrating visual stimuli such as a gaze, facial expressions, and biological motion2, and it affects daily living skills and consequently quality of life3,4. A previous study indicated that differences in sensory processing, such as abnormal sensory sensitivity and low–sensory registration, may disturb motor development and are associated with the learning of daily living skills, such as those required for bathing and using the toilet5.

Dunn proposed a theory of sensory processing explaining individual variations in behavioral response6. In Dunn’s theory, two principal axes (i.e., the neurological threshold and behavioral response) comprise the foundation of responses to sensory stimuli. The neurological threshold denotes that stimuli are slowly or quickly noticed. Additionally, a passive behavioral response allows sensory experiences to occur, inciting a reaction, followed by an active behavioral response allowing behavior engagement to control sensory input. According to these two principal axes, the response to sensory stimuli are divided into four quadrants: low registration, sensation seeking, sensory sensitivity, and sensation avoiding7. Following Dunn’s theory, the Sensory Profile Questionnaire was developed to quantify individual variations in sensory processing with respect to tendencies of behavioral responses to various sensory stimuli such as visual, auditory, touch, taste/smell, movement, and activity level8. Previous studies have also reported that sensory profiles help determine individual variations in sensory processing. Although sensory profiling is highly useful in evaluating individual variation in sensory processing, the use of questionnaires for this purpose is associated with difficulty in self–monitoring9. Individuals with low intelligence level may not be able to understand the questions while undergoing questionnaire-based sensory profiling; consequently, their sensory profile may not appropriately reflect their sensory processing. Additionally, a previous study showed that a lack of self–monitoring was observed even in individuals with normal intelligence level10.

At present, we have not established an objective evaluation method of sensory processing, but magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) has the potential to evaluate sensory processing objectively. MRI is one of the most powerful and flexible imaging tools for evaluating brain tissue. Using task-based MRI, a previous study indicated that sensory stimuli simultaneously activated multiple cortices and suggested that multiple cortices were associated with sensory processing from the perspective of integrating sensory stimuli11,12. This relationship indicates that white matter may have an important role in sensory processing because brain white matter tracts connect multiple cortices13,14. Brain white matter tracts, which are myelinated axonal fibers, connect multiple different regions such as the primary sensory cortices, higher cortices, deep gray nuclei, and brain stem15. Different white matter networks would partially lead to interindividual variability and disorders in sensory processing and resultant behavior responses16,17. Numerous studies have examined normal and abnormal connectivity and their role in developmental and acquired disorders such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD)18. The development of white matter is susceptible to environmental influences and changes19, and previous studies have suggested a relationship between sensory processing and white matter in children with sensory processing disorders16,17. Conversely, in adults, the relationship between sensory processing and white matter microstructures remains largely unknown. Moreover, white matter microstructures were shown to differ in each developmental stage and develop differently in each brain region20, suggesting that the relationship between sensory processing and white matter microstructures differs in adults.

To evaluate white matter microstructures, we used diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). Recently, using DTI to further investigate white matter anatomy has increased21. DTI is based on diffusion-weighted imaging and enables mapping of the diffusion process of water or other molecules. Using appropriate magnetic field gradients, DTI visualizes the white matter tract architecture in the direction of maximum diffusivity aligning with the direction of white matter tract orientation and their myelination, which act as barriers to the random motion of water molecules18. Moreover, DTI measures the displacement of water molecules on the micron scale and yields four diffusion parameters: fractional anisotropy (FA), axonal diffusivity (AD), radial diffusivity (RD), and mean diffusivity (MD). These diffusion parameters provide information on white matter tracts such as cell density, axons, and myelin sheath22. Here, we investigated the association between the Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile questionnaire (AASP) and DTI white matter tract architecture. In young adults, the DTI analysis revealed a positive relationship between AASP scores (i.e., sensory sensitivity, sensation avoiding, activity level)/subscores (i.e., sensory sensitivity–activity level and sensation avoiding–touch) and DTI diffusion parameters (i.e., AD and MD) in the cingulum–cingulate gyrus bundle (CCG) and between sensory sensitivity–auditory and MD in the uncinate fasciculus (UNC).

Results

Demographics

The present study included 84 typically developed young adults (40 men and 42 women [for intelligence quotient (IQ) scoring]). We could not measure the IQ of two participants. The information on participants, such as age, body mass index (BMI), and IQ, is shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics.

Correlations between AASP scores and diffusion parameters of each white matter pathway

The analysis of AASP scores and diffusion parameters demonstrated positive correlations in the right CCG (P < 0.001). The correlations and effect sizes are shown in Table 2, Figs. 1, and 2. In other brain regions, there were no significant correlations between the diffusion parameters and AASP item scores. The correlations corresponding to the right CCG were between: AD and activity level, AD and sensory sensitivity, AD and sensation avoiding, and MD and sensation avoiding. Additionally, we performed a partial correlation analysis, entering age, sex, and BMI as covariates, and found similar results.

Table 2 Relationship between diffusion parameters and AASP score.
Figure 1
figure 1

Relationship between right CCG parameters and sensory processing with regard to avoidance of a particular sensation and tactile stimulus. Regarding the right CCG, we observed a positive correlation between diffusion parameters and the AASP score (P < 0.001). (a) Correlations between the right CCG and the AASP score (sensation avoiding). (a-1) shows the correlation between AD and sensation avoiding, and (a-2) shows the correlation between MD and sensation avoiding. (b) Correlations between right CCG parameters and the subscore of AASP (sensation avoiding–touch). (b-1) shows the correlation between AD and sensation avoiding-touch, and (b-2) shows the correlation between MD and sensation avoiding-touch. The right CCG is the red region of interest selected by TRACULA. CCG cingulum–cingulate gyrus, AD axonal diffusivity, MD mean diffusivity, AASP Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile.

Figure 2
figure 2

Relationship between the CCG and sensory processing with regard to sensitivity of a particular sensation and activity level-related stimulus. Regarding the right CCG, we observed a positive correlation between diffusion parameters and the AASP score (P < 0.001). (a) Correlations between the right CCG and the AASP score (activity level and sensory sensitivity). (a-1) shows the correlation between AD and activity level, and (a-2) shows the correlation between AD and sensory sensitivity. (b) Correlation between right CCG parameters and the subscore of AASP (sensory sensitivity–activity level). Correlation between AD and sensory sensitivity–activity level. The right CCG is the red region of interest selected by TRACULA. CCG cingulum-cingulate gyrus, AD axonal diffusivity, MD mean diffusivity, AASP Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile.

Correlations between AASP subscores and diffusion parameters of each white matter pathway

Further, the analysis of AASP subscores and diffusion parameters demonstrated positive correlations in the right CCG and right UNC (P < 0.001). The correlations corresponding to the right CCG were between: AD and sensory sensitivity–activity level, AD and sensation avoiding–touch, and MD and sensation avoiding–touch. Additionally, the correlation corresponding to the right UNC was between MD and sensory sensitivity–auditory. Correlations and effect sizes are shown in Table 3 and Figs. 1, 2 and 3. In other brain regions, there were no significant correlations between diffusion parameters and AASP subscores. Additionally, we performed a partial correlation analysis, entering age, sex, and BMI as covariates, and found similar results.

Table 3 Relationship between diffusion parameters and subscores of AASP.
Figure 3
figure 3

Relationship between the right UNC and sensory processing. In the right UNC, we observed a positive correlation between MD and sensory sensitivity–auditory. The right UNC is the yellow region of interest selected by TRACULA. UNC uncinate fasciculus, MD mean diffusivity, AASP Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile.

Discussion

The present study revealed a positive correlation between diffusion parameters of the right CCG (i.e., AD, MD) and AASP scores (i.e., sensation avoiding, sensory sensitivity, activity level). Additionally, we found positive correlations between the right CCG (i.e., AD, MD) and subscores of AASP (i.e., sensation avoiding–touch, sensory sensitivity–activity level) and between MD of the right UNC and sensory sensitivity–auditory. We evaluated the effect sizes of these correlations as moderate or strong. The present study suggested that variability in white matter microstructures in the CCG and UNC may indicate low neurological thresholds for sensory stimuli in young adults.

In studies using DTI, the interpretation of diffusion parameters is important. The pathophysiology of each diffusion parameter has been revealed by previous studies23,24,25. FA is sensitive to changes in white matter microstructure23. Previous studies involving typical developed participants have demonstrated FA increases with growth, suggesting that myelin is the most influential factor of white matter microstructure anisotropy24,25,26. The present study showed no significant correlation between FA and sensory processing, suggesting there were no differences in myelin with respect to sensory processing. Additionally, RD demonstrated no significant correlation with sensory processing in the present study. This reinforces that myelin has no relationship with sensory processing, because RD is an eigenvector perpendicular to axons and is sensitive to changes in myelination25,27. We also found a correlation between AD in the CCG and sensory processing. AD is an eigenvector of diffusion parameters in the axonal direction25,27. Previous studies have suggested that AD is sensitive to axonal disassembly and that an increase in AD is caused by a decrease in the tortuosity of white matter tracts in the axonal direction, which involves changes in maturation, from tortuous to straight25,26. Thus, AD was considered to indicate tract-specific or timing-specific changes in axonal morphology25. The CCG presented a stronger decrease in axon tortuosity than other white matter regions in the course of brain development28. The present CCG results suggest that axonal changes, such as those in axon tortuosity with brain development, have an important role in the presentation of sensitivity to sensory stimuli. Furthermore, the present study showed that MD of the CCG and the UNC were correlated with sensory processing. MD is the mean of diffusivity in the axonal and perpendicular direction to axons and is affected by the size and integrity of cells23. In conjunction with the positive correlation between AD of the CCG and sensory processing, we suggest the integrity of the CCG in the axonal direction is related to sensory processing. In the UNC, we found a positive correlation between MD and sensory sensitivity–auditory. The UNC is the last region to mature during development28 and MD decreases with growth23. Thus, high MD is assumed to represent immaturity of nerve fibers, and we suggest that immaturity of the UNC is related to sensory sensitivity to auditory stimuli.

The present study showed a positive correlation between diffusion parameters of the right CCG and AASP scores/subscores. Using effect sizes based on previous studies29,30, in the CCG, almost all correlations were moderate; however, the correlation was strong for sensation avoiding–touch and sensory sensitivity–activity level. A previous study indicated that the CCG connects various areas: the cingulate cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, splenium of the corpus callosum, parahippocampal gyrus, superior frontal gyrus, precuneus, superior parietal lobule, superior occipital lobule, and supplementary motor area31. The CCG is considered a core part of the limbic system and to be involved in the processing of emotion, pain, reward, motion, conflict, error detection, and memory, which mainly reflect the involvement of the cingulate cortex32,33. Another previous study indicated that the anterior cingulate cortex and insula were activated by sensory stimuli regardless of sensory modality, such as somatosensory, auditory, or visual; new sensory stimuli increased the activation in these areas34. Among the brain areas involved in the CCG, research has suggested that the anterior cingulate cortex and insula formed a network for the detection of changes in sensory stimuli 34,35. The present results showed that diffusion parameters of the CCG were correlated with sensation avoidance and sensitivity, which are inverse behavioral responses but have low neurological thresholds for sensory stimuli according to Dunn’s theory. In conjunction with the interpretation of diffusion parameters, our results suggest that the large axonal variability in the CCG were involved in the low neurological threshold for the sensory stimuli without behavioral responses to sensory stimuli.

Moreover, we observed that CCG parameters were related to the activity level of the AASP score and sensory sensitivity–activity level of the AASP subscore. Activity level-related items measure disposition toward involvement in daily activities36 and sensory sensitivity–activity level indicates excessive response to environmental sensory stimuli in daily life and behavioral features of distractedness. The environmental sensory stimuli represented by activity level consist of multisensory stimuli such as visual and auditory stimuli in daily life. A previous study suggested that multisensory processing was related to the brain area centered at the CCG37. Additional previous studies have suggested that the CCG is related to attention to the surroundings in daily life and sustained attention38,39,40. Considering this relationship between the CCG and multisensory processing or attention and diffusion parameters, our results suggest that large axonal change in the CCG are related to a low neurological threshold to environmental stimuli in daily life and control of attention to sensory stimuli.

Additionally, we demonstrated that the CCG had a relationship with sensation avoiding–touch. Touch processing-related items in the AASP measure responses to tactile stimuli on the skin. The relationship between the CCG and tactile stimuli was previously observed in gentle touch-induced activities in the somatosensory, motor area, and cingulate regions41 and non-self/self-produced tactile stimulus-induced/reduced activities in the cingulate gyrus42. Thus, in conjunction with previous findings, we suggest that CCG parameters are related to a low neurological threshold for processing of physical touch.

In the present study, a right UNC parameter demonstrated a moderate correlation with sensory sensitivity–auditory. The UNC not only connects the orbitofrontal cortex and temporal lobe anatomically, but also is considered the key pathway associated with episodic memory, language, and social emotional processing43,44. A recent meta-analysis of 25 DTI-based studies indicated that individuals with ASD showed decreased FA in the UNC, thus supporting the theory of specific underconnectivity in patients with ASD who show difficulty in social auditory information and language processing45. Furthermore, a previous mouse model showed that the UNC was anatomically connected to the primary auditory cortex and activated by sound stimuli and stimuli to the orbitofrontal cortex. Thus, the UNC and auditory processing are closely intertwined. Auditory processing items measure response to stimuli that are heard by an individual. Sensory sensitivity–auditory is associated with high sensitivity to auditory stimuli, leading to the presentation of excessive behavioral responses and disturbing behavior36. Our present results suggest that the UNC is a core white matter connection between the orbitofrontal cortex and primary auditory cortex and of the interaction related to sensitivity to auditory stimuli and behavioral response.

The present study has several limitations. First, the effect of age should be considered. Previous studies have shown that the maturity of white matter differs in each brain area and that the CCG and UNC develop later than other white matter tracts33,46. The peak of CCG FA was observed at 42 years of age; therefore, the CCG was considered one of the white matter tracts that mature at a later stage46. Additionally, the UNC is one of the last developing tracts, and it continues maturing into the third decade of life47. The maturational peak of the UNC is extended throughout adolescence and young adulthood and peaks beyond the age of 30 years46. Considering that the present study targeted young adulthood, the present results suggest that the degree of maturity of the CCG and UNC may affect sensory processing in young adulthood. In the present study, although we considered age as a control variable, it should be noted that the age range was 19–39 years with an average of 24.5 years. Our results should not be applied to other developmental stages. Notably, previous studies where the ages of the subjects were 8–12 or 8–11 years showed that there was a relationship between sensory processing and posterior white matter related to motor and sensory areas, which contrasts our lack of results16,17. Considering that the maturation speed of white matter is different in each brain area, this finding does not contradict the hypothesis that brain areas related to sensory processing differ at each development stage.

Additionally, there was no psychometric support for the use of the AASP subscores. The AASP is designed to evaluate sensory processing in four quadrants and six modalities. Thus, we should use caution when interpreting the AASP subscores. Certainly, the AASP is a useful tool to examine sensory processing, but the AASP score cannot explain, in detail, the effect of a modality on a quadrant. Because using the AASP as originally designed is insufficient to research sensory processing in detail, we evaluated sensory processing in using the AASP subscores.

In future studies, because our results can only be generalized to young, typically developed adults, we should conduct research regarding disorders that affect sensory processing, such as ASD. ASD is a complex neurodevelopmental condition with variable deficits in social behavior and language, restrictive interests, repetitive behaviors, and sensory processing48,49. It has been reported that 94% of individual with ASD show sensory symptoms, which are suggested to have a central role in other symptoms, such as social–communication deficits and repetitive behaviors48,49. Considering that differences in white matter microstructure have been reported in ASD50, our results in young, typically developed adults may not be applicable to this population. The relationship between sensory processing and white matter microstructures using AASP and DTI in a sample of individuals with ASD needs to be examined. As differences between ASD and typical development have been indicated in the CCG and UNC50, we hypothesize that ASD would demonstrate different relationships between sensory processing and white matter microstructures from those reported here.

The present study revealed the relationship between sensory processing and CCG and UNC white matter parameters. We suggest that in young adults, the axonal tortuosity of the CCG represents a low neurological threshold regardless of behavioral responses to sensory stimuli and that the cell integrity of the UNC is related to sensory sensitivity to auditory stimuli. While we need more studies involving participants in other development stages to interpret the diffusion tensor index, our current study has provided new information on the neurological basis of sensory processing.

Methods

Participants

The present study included 84 typically developed individuals (42 men and 42 women); they were right handed, as assessed with the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory51. Participants who were using psychiatric medications at the time of evaluation or had a history of brain injury, major medical conditions, or alcohol or drug dependence were excluded. Full-scale IQ scores were measured using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III. Our study was performed in accordance with the Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act guidelines, was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of University of Fukui, and all participants provided written informed consent. Typically developed participants were interviewed by trained psychiatrists (H.K., I.O) to screen for the presence of neuropsychiatric disorders using the Structured Clinical Interview for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition—Axis I Disorders52.

AASP

We used the Japanese version of the AASP for the evaluation of sensory processing53. The AASP was standardized in Japanese and showed comparable psychometric properties with the original instrument53,54. The AASP is a standardized self-report questionnaire that includes 60 items in total and employs a five-point Likert scale. The AASP consists of sensory processing modalities and quadrants and includes six modalities, namely visual, auditory, touch, smell/taste, movement, and activity level, composed of 10, 11, 13, 8, 8, and 10 items, respectively. The sum of scores of modality-specific items was determined; the higher the total score of a modality, the greater the corresponding atypical tendencies of sensory processing. Moreover, the AASP includes four quadrant scores, each consisting of 15 items: low registration, sensation seeking, sensory sensitivity, and sensation avoiding. The quadrant states are based on Dunn’s sensory processing model36, which classifies the tendency of responses to sensory stimuli in terms of a neurological threshold and behavioral responses. Low registration refers to the tendency of showing a high neurological threshold and passive behavioral responses, associated with sensory bluntness where even strong sensory stimuli may go unnoticed. This tendency is associated with a delay in response to sensory stimuli and often with passive behavioral response. Sensation seeking refers to the tendency of showing a high neurological threshold and active behavioral responses. Sensation seeking requires specific sensory stimuli to satisfy a high neurological threshold and stabilize an easily bored state without specific sensory stimuli. Sensory sensitivity refers to the tendency of showing a low neurological threshold, receiving strong stimuli, and feeling pain even after exposure to mild sensory stimuli because of passive behavioral responses. Sensation avoiding refers to the tendency of showing low neurological thresholds and avoiding unpleasant sensory stimuli because of active behavioral responses. In a previous standardization study in Japanese version of the AASP, typical sensory processing was demonstrated in 16–84% of the research sample53. The cutoff scores for participants aged 18–34 years were 23–38 in low registration, 30–47 in sensation seeking, 25–42 in sensory sensitivity, and 25–41 in sensation avoiding53. The cutoff scores for participants aged 35–64 years old were 20–34 in low registration, 29–45 in sensation seeking, 22–39 in sensory sensitivity, and 22–39 in sensation avoiding53. We found that the mean for all quadrant scores were within the cutoff score. Although four participants were beyond the cutoff scores, we included all participants in our statistical analysis because all participants were evaluated to be typically developed by trained psychiatrists (H.K., I.O). Information on each modality-specific and quadrant score (mean, standard deviation, range, and possible score) of participants is shown in Table 1.

To study sensory processing in further detail, we subdivided modality-specific items according to quadrant. For example, for the item “I am distracted if there is a lot of noise around”, the corresponding quadrant modality is sensory sensitivity–auditory. The sum of quadrant-specific modality scores was considered the subscore. There were 24 subscores, which had 1–4 items each. The sum of the subscores was determined; the higher the total subscore, the greater the quadrant tendencies of the modality-specific sensory processing.

MRI acquisition

MR images were acquired using a 3-Tesla positron emission tomography (PET)/MR scanner (SIGNA PET/MR; General Electric Medical Systems, Milwaukee, WI, USA) with an 8-channel head coil. High-resolution T1-weighted anatomical MRI and DTI were acquired using the scan parameters of our previous study55. The detailed scan parameters were: T1-weighted anatomical MRI (repetition time = 6.38 ms, echo time (TE) = 1.99 ms, flip angle = 11°, field of view = 256 mm, number of slices, 172, voxel dimension = 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.0 mm3); DTI (single–shot echo–planar imaging, acquisition matrix = 128 × 128, TE = minimum, repetition time = 9327 ms, field of view = 240 mm, pixel size = 1.9 mm × 1.9 mm2, number of axial slices = 45, slice thickness/gap = 3.0 mm/0 mm, 30 distributed isotropic orientations, b–values of 1000 s/mm2 and 0).

DTI analysis

We used TRActs Constrained by UnderLying Anatomy (TRACULA56) for the analysis of diffusion–weighted images. TRACULA is an automated global probabilistic tractography tool in FreeSurfer (version 6.0; http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). First, we reconstructed each participant’s T1-weighted anatomical image and obtained cortical and subcortical regions using FreeSurfer57. Subsequently, we used TRACULA to reconstruct volumetric distributions of 18 major white matter pathways in the participants’ diffusion-weighted data. The automated tractography method relies on prior anatomical information based on a set of training subjects, which was obtained with manual labeling. The 18 reconstructed white matter pathways were as follows: the bilateral corticospinal tract, bilateral inferior longitudinal fasciculus, bilateral UNC, bilateral anterior thalamic radiations, bilateral CCG, bilateral cingulum–angular bundle, bilateral superior longitudinal fasciculus–parietal terminations, bilateral superior longitudinal fasciculus–temporal terminations, corpus callosum–forceps major, and corpus callosum–forceps minor. Finally, we extracted four diffusion parameters (FA, MD, RD, and AD) from each white matter pathway using the “ball-and-stick” model of diffusion. The mean values of diffusion measures were calculated by thresholding each pathway distribution at 20% of its maximum value for each participant. To quantify head motion in each participant’s diffusion-weighted data, we calculated four measures of head motion: translation (mm), rotation (degree), average signal intensity dropout score, and portion of slices with greater than the computed dropout score (%) and removed eddy current-induced image distortions58.

Statistical analysis

We performed partial correlation analysis using SPSS (version 20; IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) to evaluate the strength of the relationship between the AASP score (six modality-specific scores, four quadrant scores) and diffusion measures (FA, MD, AD, and RD) of each white matter pathway, by considering age, BMI, and sex-specific differences as control variables. Additionally, to examine the AASP in further detail, we performed a partial correlation analysis to evaluate the relationship between the AASP subscores (24 items; low registration–visual, low registration–auditory, etc.) and diffusion parameters of each white matter pathway using age, BMI, and sex differences as control variables. In all partial correlation analyses, P < 0.001 was considered statistically significant. Additionally, we calculated Fisher’s Zr of the statistically significant correlations as the effect size59 and evaluated effect sizes based on previous studies29,30.